Register      Login
Emu Emu Society
Journal of BirdLife Australia
RESEARCH ARTICLE

The vocal repertoire of the cooperatively breeding Apostlebird (Struthidea cinerea)

Miyako H. Warrington A D , Paul G. McDonald B , Aliza K. Sager A and Simon C. Griffith A C E
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Department of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia.

B Behavioural and Physiological Ecology Research Centre, Zoology, School of Environmental and Rural Sciences, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia.

C School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia.

D Present address: Natural Resources Institute, University of Manitoba, 70 Dysart Road, Winnipeg, MB, R3T 2N2, Canada.

E Corresponding author. Email: simon.griffith@mq.edu.au

Emu 114(3) 206-221 https://doi.org/10.1071/MU13051
Submitted: 12 June 2013  Accepted: 16 January 2014   Published: 5 June 2014

Abstract

The role of acoustic communication in facilitating social interactions and mediating cooperative behaviour has been highlighted by many studies. In the ‘social complexity hypothesis’ of communication, many more specialised signals are likely to evolve in social species where many individuals interact in multiple behaviours. However, before the function of vocalisations in these systems can be elucidated accurately, the characteristics and social context of each vocalisation must be determined. Apostlebirds (Struthidea cinerea) are a highly vocal and social species with an obligate cooperatively breeding life-history. In this study, we describe the environmental and behavioural context of the 17 most common calls from the vocal repertoire of a study population of Apostlebirds in north-western New South Wales. The vocalisations given by individuals ranged from simple, monosyllabic calls through to more complex calls with multiple syllables and frequency modulations. All these calls were broadly categorised as close-range, long-range, alarm context, or associated with nesting. Most call-types were given by both sexes, and by both breeders and helpers, including alarm calls that were given by both juveniles and adults. In contrast, calls given during inter-group interactions were predominantly by adults.

Additional keywords: avian acoustics, call analysis, Corcoracidae, sociality.


References

Baker, M. C. (2004). The chorus song of cooperatively breeding Laughing Kookaburras (Coraciiformes, Halcyonidae : Dacelo novaeguineae): characterization and comparison among groups. Ethology 110, 21–35.
The chorus song of cooperatively breeding Laughing Kookaburras (Coraciiformes, Halcyonidae : Dacelo novaeguineae): characterization and comparison among groups.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Baker, M. C. (2009). Information content in chorus songs of the group-living Australian Magpie (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis) in Western Australia. Ethology 115, 227–238.
Information content in chorus songs of the group-living Australian Magpie (Cracticus tibicen dorsalis) in Western Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Baldwin, M. (1974). Studies of the Apostle Bird at Inverell. Part I. General behaviour. Sunbird 5, 77–88.

Baldwin, M. (1975). Studies of the Apostle Bird at Inverell. Part II. Breeding behaviour. Sunbird 6, 1–7.

Beecher, M. D., Beecher, I. M., and Hahn, S. (1981). Parent–offspring recognition in Bank Swallows (Riparia riparia): II. Development and acoustic basis. Animal Behaviour 29, 95–101.
Parent–offspring recognition in Bank Swallows (Riparia riparia): II. Development and acoustic basis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Black, J. M. (1988). Preflight signalling in swans: a mechanism for group cohesion and flock formation. Ethology 79, 143–157.
Preflight signalling in swans: a mechanism for group cohesion and flock formation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Blumstein, D. T., and Armitage, K. B. (1997). Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A comparative test with ground dwelling sciurid alarm calls. American Naturalist 150, 179–200.
Does sociality drive the evolution of communicative complexity? A comparative test with ground dwelling sciurid alarm calls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD1cnit1OlsA%3D%3D&md5=6c539e516ab15d2fe4ac0c98ca512de2CAS | 18811281PubMed |

Bousquet, C. A. H., Sumpter, D. J. T., and Manser, M. B. (2010). Moving calls: a vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London – B. Biological Sciences 278(1711), 1482–1488.

Brown, E. D., and Farabaugh, S. M. (1991). Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian Magpie, Gymnorhina Tibicen. Part III. Sex specificity and individual specificity of vocal parts in communal chorus and duet songs. Behaviour 118, 244–274.
Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian Magpie, Gymnorhina Tibicen. Part III. Sex specificity and individual specificity of vocal parts in communal chorus and duet songs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Brown, E. D., Farabaugh, S. M., and Veltman, C. J. (1988). Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian Magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen. Part I. Vocal sharing within and among social groups. Behaviour 104, 1–27.
Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian Magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen. Part I. Vocal sharing within and among social groups.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Catchpole, C. K., and Slater, P. J. B. (2008). ‘Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations’, 2nd edn. (Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.)

Chamberlain, D. R., and Cornwell, G. W. (1971). Selected vocalizations of the Common Crow. Auk 88, 613–634.

Chapman, G. (1998). The social life of the Apostlebird Struthidea cinerea. Emu 98, 178–183.
The social life of the Apostlebird Struthidea cinerea.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Charif, R., Strickman, L., and Waack, A. (2008). ‘Raven Pro 1.3 User’s Manual.’ (Cornell Lab of Ornithology: Ithaca, NY.)

Clutton-Brock, T. (2002). Breeding together: kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. Science 296, 69–72.
Breeding together: kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD38XivVSqs70%3D&md5=4d001b273c3aa817d3f306ecb686dc4aCAS | 11935014PubMed |

Falls, J. B. (1982). Individual recognition by sounds in birds. In ‘Acoustic Communication in Birds’. (Eds D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller.) pp. 237–278. (Academic Press: New York.)

Freeberg, T. M., Dunbar, R. I. M., and Ord, T. J. (2012). Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 367, 1785–1801.
Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22641818PubMed |

Goodale, E., and Podos, J. (2010). Persistence of song types in Darwin’s Finches, Geospiza fortis, over four decades. Biology Letters 6, 589–592.
Persistence of song types in Darwin’s Finches, Geospiza fortis, over four decades.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20392717PubMed |

Greig, E., and Pruett-Jones, S. (2008). Splendid songs: the vocal behaviour of Splendid Fairy-wrens (Malurus splendens melanotus). Emu 108, 103–114.
Splendid songs: the vocal behaviour of Splendid Fairy-wrens (Malurus splendens melanotus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Griesser, M., Barnaby, J., Schneider, N. A., Figenschau, N., Wright, J., Griffith, S. C., Kazem, A., and Russell, A. F. (2009). Influence of winter ranging behaviour on the social organization of a cooperatively breeding bird species, the Apostlebird. Ethology 115, 888–896.
Influence of winter ranging behaviour on the social organization of a cooperatively breeding bird species, the Apostlebird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Griffiths, R., Daan, S., and Dijkstra, C. (1996). Sex identification in birds using two CHD genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 263, 1251–1256.
Sex identification in birds using two CHD genes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DyaK28XmsFCitLw%3D&md5=db2a8f4429629efa936d6951f8b8cbc6CAS |

Higgins, P. J., Peter, J. M., and Cowling, S. J. (2006). Apostlebird. In ‘Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic Birds. Vol. 7: Boatbill to Starlings’. (Eds P. J. Higgins, J. M. Peter, S. J. Cowling.) pp. 797–815. (Oxford University Press: Melbourne.)

Halpin, Z. T. (1991). Kin recognition cues of vertebrates. In ‘Kin Recognition’. (Ed. P.G. Hepper.) pp. 220–258. (Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.)

Hill, B. G., and Lein, M. R. (1985). The non-song vocal repertoire of the White-crowned Sparrow. Condor 87, 327–335.
The non-song vocal repertoire of the White-crowned Sparrow.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Janik, V. M. (1999). Pitfalls in the categorization of behaviour: a comparison of dolphin whistle classification methods. Animal Behaviour 57, 133–143.
Pitfalls in the categorization of behaviour: a comparison of dolphin whistle classification methods.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10053080PubMed |

Johnson, F. R., McNaughton, E. J., Shelley, C. D., and Blumstein, D. T. (2003). Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls. Australian Journal of Zoology 51, 577–585.
Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kalinowski, S. T., Taper, M. L., and Marshall, T. C. (2007). Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment. Molecular Ecology 16, 1099–1106.
Revising how the computer program CERVUS accommodates genotyping error increases success in paternity assignment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17305863PubMed |

Kennedy, R. A. W., Evans, C. S., and McDonald, P. G. (2009). Individual distinctiveness in the mobbing call of a cooperative bird, the Noisy Miner Manorina melanocephala. Journal of Avian Biology 40, 481–490.
Individual distinctiveness in the mobbing call of a cooperative bird, the Noisy Miner Manorina melanocephala.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Krams, I., Krama, T., Freeberg, T. M., Kullberg, C., and Lucas, J. R. (2012). Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 367, 1879–1891.
Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22641826PubMed |

Marler, P. (2004). Bird calls: a cornucopia for communication. In ‘Nature’s Music: The Science of Birdsong’. (Ed. P. S. H. Marler.) pp. 132–177 (Elsevier Academic Press: San Diego, CA.)

Mathevon, N., Koralek, A., Weldele, M., Glickman, S. E., and Theunissen, F. (2010). What the hyena’s laugh tells: sex, age, dominance and individual signature in the giggling call of Crocuta crocuta. BMC Ecology 10, 9.
What the hyena’s laugh tells: sex, age, dominance and individual signature in the giggling call of Crocuta crocuta.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 20353550PubMed |

McDonald, P. G. (2012). Cooperative bird differentiates between the calls of different individuals, even when vocalizations were from completely unfamiliar individuals. Biology Letters 8, 365–368.
Cooperative bird differentiates between the calls of different individuals, even when vocalizations were from completely unfamiliar individuals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22258445PubMed |

McDonald, P. G., and Wright, J. (2011). Bell Miner provisioning calls are more similar among relatives and are used by helpers at the nest to bias their effort towards kin. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 278, 3403–3411.
Bell Miner provisioning calls are more similar among relatives and are used by helpers at the nest to bias their effort towards kin.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

McDonald, P. G., Heathcote, C. F., Clarke, M. F., Wright, J., and Kazem, A. J. N. (2007). Provisioning calls of the cooperatively breeding Bell Miner Manorina melanophrys encode sufficient information for individual discrimination. Journal of Avian Biology 38, 113–121.
Provisioning calls of the cooperatively breeding Bell Miner Manorina melanophrys encode sufficient information for individual discrimination.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L., Rowley, I., and Russell, E. M. (1991). Social recognition and response to song in cooperative Red-winged Fairy-wrens. Auk 108, 811–819.

Podos, J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes : Emberizidae). Evolution 51, 537–551.
A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes : Emberizidae).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Pollard, K. A., and Blumstein, D. T. (2012). Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 367, 1869–1878.
Evolving communicative complexity: insights from rodents and beyond.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 22641825PubMed |

Popa-Lisseanu, A. G., Bontadina, F., Mora, O., and Ibanez, C. (2008). Highly structured fission–fusion societies in an aerial-hawking, carnivorous bat. Animal Behaviour 75, 471–482.
Highly structured fission–fusion societies in an aerial-hawking, carnivorous bat.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Price, J. J. (1999). Recognition of family-specific calls in Stripe-backed Wrens. Animal Behaviour 57, 483–492.
Recognition of family-specific calls in Stripe-backed Wrens.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10049489PubMed |

Radford, A. N. (2004). Vocal mediation of foraging competition in the cooperatively breeding Green Woodhoopoe (Phoeniculus purpureus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56, 279–285.
Vocal mediation of foraging competition in the cooperatively breeding Green Woodhoopoe (Phoeniculus purpureus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Radford, A. N., and Ridley, A. R. (2008). Close calling regulates spacing between foraging competitors in the group-living Pied Babbler. Animal Behaviour 75, 519–527.
Close calling regulates spacing between foraging competitors in the group-living Pied Babbler.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Reyer, H. U., and Schmidl, D. (1988). Helpers have little to laugh about: group structure and vocalisation in the Laughing Kookaburra Dacelo novaeguineae. Emu 88, 150–160.
Helpers have little to laugh about: group structure and vocalisation in the Laughing Kookaburra Dacelo novaeguineae.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rollins, L. A., Holleley, C. E., Wright, J., Russell, A. F., and Griffith, S. C. (2010). Isolation and characterization of 12 polymorphic tetranucleotide microsatellite loci in the Apostlebird (Struthidea cinerea). Conservation Genetics Resources 2, 229–231.

Searcy, W. A. (1992). Song repertoire and mate choice in birds. American Zoologist 32, 71–80.

Sethi, V. K., and Bhatt, D. (2008). Call repertoire of an endemic avian species, the Indian Chat Cercomela fusca. Current Science 94, 1173–1179.

Sharp, S. P., and Hatchwell, B. J. (2005). Individuality in the contact calls of cooperatively breeding Long-tailed Tits (Aegithalos caudatus). Behaviour 142, 1559–1575.
Individuality in the contact calls of cooperatively breeding Long-tailed Tits (Aegithalos caudatus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Smith, W. J., and Smith, A. M. (1996). Vocal signalling of the Great Crested Flycatcher, Myiarchus crinitus (Aves, Tyrannidae). Ethology 102, 705–723.

Snowdon, C. T., and Hausberger, M. (1997). ‘Social Influences on Vocal Development.’ (Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.)

Stoddard, P. K. (1996). Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. In ‘Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds’. (Eds D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller.) pp. 356–374. (Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY.)

Tibbetts, E. A., and Dale, J. (2007). Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 22, 529–537.
Individual recognition: it is good to be different.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Townsend, S. W., Hollen, L. I., and Manser, M. B. (2010). Meerkat close calls encode group-specific signatures, but receivers fail to discriminate. Animal Behaviour 80, 133–138.
Meerkat close calls encode group-specific signatures, but receivers fail to discriminate.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Trainer, J. M., and McDonald, D. B. (1993). Vocal repertoire of the Long-tailed Manakin and its relation to male–male cooperation. Condor 95, 769–781.
Vocal repertoire of the Long-tailed Manakin and its relation to male–male cooperation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Warrington, M. H., Rollins, L. A., Raihani, N. J., Russell, A. F., and Griffith, S. C. (2013). Genetic monogamy despite variable ecological conditions and social environment in the cooperatively breeding Apostlebird. Ecology & Evolution 3, 4669–4682.
Genetic monogamy despite variable ecological conditions and social environment in the cooperatively breeding Apostlebird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Whitmore, M. J. (1981). Egg predation and possible usurption of an Australian Magpie-lark’s nest by Apostlebirds. Emu 81, 111–112.
Egg predation and possible usurption of an Australian Magpie-lark’s nest by Apostlebirds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Woxvold, I. A. (2004). Breeding ecology and group dynamics of the Apostlebird. Australian Journal of Zoology 52, 561–581.
Breeding ecology and group dynamics of the Apostlebird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Woxvold, I. A., and Magrath, M. J. L. (2005). Helping enhances multiple components of reproductive success in the cooperatively breeding Apostlebird. Journal of Animal Ecology 74, 1039–1050.
Helping enhances multiple components of reproductive success in the cooperatively breeding Apostlebird.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Woxvold, I. A., and Mulder, R. A. (2008). Mixed mating strategies in cooperatively breeding Apostlebirds Struthidea cinerea. Journal of Avian Biology 39, 50–56.
Mixed mating strategies in cooperatively breeding Apostlebirds Struthidea cinerea.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |