Register      Login
Wildlife Research Wildlife Research Society
Ecology, management and conservation in natural and modified habitats
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Observer effects occur when estimating alert but not flight-initiation distances

P.-J. Guay A B , E. M. McLeod A , R. Cross A , A. J. Formby C , S. P. Maldonado A , R. E. Stafford-Bell A , Z. N. St-James-Turner A , R. W. Robinson A , R. A. Mulder C and M. A. Weston D E
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A Applied Ecology Research Group and Institute for Sustainability and Innovation, College of Engineering and Science, Victoria University – St Albans Campus, PO Box 14428, Melbourne MC, Vic. 8001, Australia.

B College of Health and Biomedicine, Victoria University – St Albans Campus, PO Box 14428, Melbourne MC, Vic. 8001, Australia.

C Department of Zoology, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Vic. 3010, Australia.

D Centre for Integrative Ecology, School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, Vic. 3125, Australia.

E Corresponding author. Email: mweston@deakin.edu.au

Wildlife Research 40(4) 289-293 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR13013
Submitted: 22 January 2013  Accepted: 20 May 2013   Published: 19 June 2013

Abstract

Context: The estimation of alert (vigilance) and flight-initiation (escape) distances (AD and FID, respectively) has underpinned theoretical and applied studies of the escape behaviour and management of disturbance to wildlife. Many studies use multiple observers, and some conduct meta-analyses; these efforts assume no observer effects in the estimation of these distances.

Aims and methods: We compared the estimates of FID and AD under ideal conditions (i.e. of black swans, Cygnus atratus, a large species with obvious behaviour, and at a location where swans allowed close approaches in open habitats), by one experienced and four inexperienced observers.

Key results: FID did not differ among observers but AD differed between the experienced and all inexperienced observers, and among inexperienced observers. Thus, FID estimates appear more repeatable than those of AD. Experience apparently results in more conservative estimates of AD.

Conclusions: FID represents a repeatable measure that is consistent across observers. This study supports its broad application in the study of wildlife escape behaviour.

Implications: We recommend the use of FID rather than AD for comparative analyses that involve multiple observers, because FID is more reliably measured.

Additional keywords: disturbance, FID, flush, pedestrian, vigilance, walker, waterfowl, wetland.


References

Blumstein, D. T. (2003). Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependant on intruder starting distance. The Journal of Wildlife Management 67, 852–857.
Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependant on intruder starting distance.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Blumstein, D. T. (2006). Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Animal Behaviour 71, 389–399.
Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: how life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Burger, J., Gochfeld, M., Jenkins, C. D., and Lesser, F. (2010). Effect of approaching boats on nesting black skimmers: using response distances to establish protective buffer zones. The Journal of Wildlife Management 74, 102–108.
Effect of approaching boats on nesting black skimmers: using response distances to establish protective buffer zones.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Chamaillé-Jammes, S., and Blumstein, D. T. (2012). A case for quantile regression in behavioral ecology: getting more out of flight initiation distance data. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66, 985–992.
A case for quantile regression in behavioral ecology: getting more out of flight initiation distance data.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cunningham, R. B., Lindenmayer, D. B., Nix, H. A., and Lindenmayer, B. D. (1999). Quantifying observer heterogeneity in bird counts. Australian Journal of Ecology 24, 270–277.
Quantifying observer heterogeneity in bird counts.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Eason, P. K., Sherman, P. T., Rankin, O., and Coleman, B. (2006). Factors influencing flight initiation distance in American Robin. The Journal of Wildlife Management 70, 1796–1800.
Factors influencing flight initiation distance in American Robin.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fernández-Juricic, E., Jimenez, M. D., and Lucas, E. (2001). Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance: implications for park design. Environmental Conservation 28, 263–269.
Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance: implications for park design.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fernández-Juricic, E., Jimenez, M. D., and Lucas, E. (2002). Factors affecting intra- and inter-specific variations in the difference between alert distances and flight distances for birds in forested habitats. Canadian Journal of Zoology 80, 1212–1220.
Factors affecting intra- and inter-specific variations in the difference between alert distances and flight distances for birds in forested habitats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Glover, H. K., Weston, M. A., Maguire, G. S., Miller, K. K., and Christie, B. A. (2011). Towards ecologically meaningful and socially acceptable buffers: response distances of shorebirds in Victoria, Australia, to human disturbance. Landscape and Urban Planning 103, 326–334.
Towards ecologically meaningful and socially acceptable buffers: response distances of shorebirds in Victoria, Australia, to human disturbance.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Goss-Custard, J. D., Cayford, J. T., Boates, J. S., and le V. dit Durell, S. E. A. (1987). Field tests of the accuracy of estimating prey size from bill length in oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, eating mussels, Mytilus edulis. Animal Behaviour 35, 1078–1083.
Field tests of the accuracy of estimating prey size from bill length in oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, eating mussels, Mytilus edulis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Guay, P.-J., and Mulder, R. A. (2009). Do neck-collars affect the behaviour and condition of black swans (Cygnus atratus)? Emu 109, 248–251.
Do neck-collars affect the behaviour and condition of black swans (Cygnus atratus)?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Guay, P.-J., Weston, M. A., Symonds, M. R. E., and Glover, H. K. (2013a). Brains and bravery: little evidence of a relationship between brain size and flightiness in shorebirds. Austral Ecology , .

Guay, P.-J., Lorenz, R. D. A., Robinson, R. W., Symonds, M. R. E., and Weston, M. A. (2013b). Distance from water, sex and approach direction influence flight distances among habituated black swans. Ethology 119, 552–558.

Hediger, H. (1934). Zur Biologie und Psychologie der Flucht bei Tieren. Biologisches Zentralblatt 54, 21–40.

Hill, D., Hockin, D., Price, D., Tucker, G., Morris, R., and Treweek, J. (1997). Bird disturbance: improving the quality and utility of disturbance research. Journal of Applied Ecology 34, 275–288.
Bird disturbance: improving the quality and utility of disturbance research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Lima, S. L., and Bednekoff, P. A. (1999). Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack? Animal Behaviour 58, 537–543.
Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10479369PubMed |

Møller, A. P., and Erritzøe, J. (2010). Flight distance and eye size in birds. Ethology 116, 458–465.
Flight distance and eye size in birds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Monie, L. (2011) Factors affecting alert distance and flight-initiation distance in black swans (Cygnus atratus) at Albert Park Lake, Victoria, Australia. Third Year Industry Project Thesis, Victoria University, St Albans, Melbourne.

Mulder, R. A., Guay, P.-J., Wilson, M., and Coulson, G. (2010). Citizen science: recruiting residents for studies of tagged urban wildlife. Wildlife Research 37, 440–446.
Citizen science: recruiting residents for studies of tagged urban wildlife.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

O’Neal Campbell, M. (2006). Urban parks as shared spaces? The utility of alert distances as indicators of avian tolerance of humans in Stirling, Scotland. Area 38, 301–311.
Urban parks as shared spaces? The utility of alert distances as indicators of avian tolerance of humans in Stirling, Scotland.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Payne, C. J., Jessop, T. J., Guay, P.-J., Johnstone, M., Feore, M., and Mulder, R. A. (2012). Population, behavioural and physiological responses of an urban population of black swans to an intense annual noise event. PLoS ONE 7, e45014.
Population, behavioural and physiological responses of an urban population of black swans to an intense annual noise event.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC38XhsVSjtbjI&md5=88b63757468693fdacf9c9850fb06211CAS | 23024783PubMed |

Rodgers, J. A., and Smith, H. T. (1997). Buffer zone distances to protect foraging and loafing waterbirds from human disturbance in Florida. Wildlife Society Bulletin 25, 139–145.

Stankowich, T., and Blumstein, D. T. (2005). Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 272, 2627–2634.
Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Van Der Meer, J., and Camphuysen, C. J. (1996). Effect of observer differences on abundance estimates of seabirds from ship-based strip transect surveys. The Ibis 138, 433–437.
Effect of observer differences on abundance estimates of seabirds from ship-based strip transect surveys.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Verner, J., and Milne, K. A. (1990). Analyst and observer variability in density estimates from spot mapping. The Condor 92, 313–325.
Analyst and observer variability in density estimates from spot mapping.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Weston, M. A., McLeod, E. M., Blumstein, D. T., and Guay, P.-J. (2012). A review of flight-initiation distances and their application to managing disturbance to Australian birds. Emu 112, 269–286.
A review of flight-initiation distances and their application to managing disturbance to Australian birds.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |