CSIRO Publishing blank image blank image blank image blank imageBooksblank image blank image blank image blank imageJournalsblank image blank image blank image blank imageAbout Usblank image blank image blank image blank imageShopping Cartblank image blank image blank image You are here: Journals > Australian Journal of Zoology   
Australian Journal of Zoology
Journal Banner
  Evolutionary, Molecular and Comparative Zoology
 
blank image Search
 
blank image blank image
blank image
 
  Advanced Search
   

Journal Home
About the Journal
Editorial Board
Contacts
Content
Online Early
Current Issue
Just Accepted
All Issues
Special Issues
Sample Issue
For Authors
General Information
Notice to Authors
Submit Article
Open Access
For Referees
Referee Guidelines
Review an Article
Annual Referee Index
For Subscribers
Subscription Prices
Customer Service
Print Publication Dates

blue arrow e-Alerts
blank image
Subscribe to our Email Alert or RSS feeds for the latest journal papers.

red arrow Connect with us
blank image
facebook twitter youtube

red arrow Supplementary Series
blank image
All volumes of the Australian Journal of Zoology Supplementary Series are online and available to subscribers of Australian Journal of Zoology.

 

Article << Previous     |     Next >>   Contents Vol 61(4)

Individual traits influence vigilance in wild female eastern grey kangaroos

A. M. Edwards A B C , E. C. Best A , S. P. Blomberg A and A. W. Goldizen A

A The University of Queensland, School of Biological Sciences, Brisbane, Qld 4072, Australia.
B Present address: The University of Tasmania, School of Zoology, Sandy Bay, Tas. 7005, Australia.
C Corresponding author. Email: amy.edwards@uqconnect.edu.au

Australian Journal of Zoology 61(4) 332-341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/ZO13025
Submitted: 19 March 2013  Accepted: 8 September 2013   Published: 26 September 2013


 
PDF (412 KB) $25
 Export Citation
 Print
  
Abstract

Vigilance is an essential component of antipredator behaviour and is also used to monitor conspecifics, but is traded off against feeding in herbivores. This trade-off can be influenced by variation in many environmental, social and individual traits. Our aim was to test the relationship between individual-level traits, including boldness, body condition and reproductive state, and vigilance, while controlling for environmental and social variables. Using multiple 5-min video samples of 30 foraging, individually recognisable, female eastern grey kangaroos (Macropus giganteus) at Sundown National Park in Queensland, we investigated individual-level variation in the duration, intensity and target of vigilance behaviour during foraging. On separate occasions, we used flight-initiation distance tests to measure boldness in our kangaroos. Females with longer flight-initiation distances (shyer females) spent more time vigilant, providing preliminary support for studies of animal personality that have suggested that boldness may covary with vigilance. Body condition did not affect the total time spent vigilant, but females in poorer body condition spent more of their vigilance time in low-intensity vigilance. Vigilance patterns were not related to reproductive state, but varied among months and differed between mornings and afternoons, and females spent more time in high-intensity vigilance when further from cover. Even after accounting for all our variables we found that 7% of the variation in total time vigilant and 14% of the variation in vigilance intensity was explained by individual identity. This highlights the importance of individual-level variation in vigilance behaviour.



Additional keywords: antipredator behaviour, boldness, group-size effect, personality.


References

Baayen, R. H. (2011). LanguageR: Data sets and functions with ‘Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics’. R package version 1.2. Available at: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=languageR

Baayen, R., Davidson, D., and Bates, D. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390–412.
CrossRef |

Bachman, G. C. (1993). The effect of body condition on the trade-off between vigilance and foraging in Belding’s ground squirrels. Animal Behaviour 46, 233–244.
CrossRef |

Banks, P. B. (2001). Predation-sensitive grouping and habitat use by eastern grey kangaroos: a field experiment. Animal Behaviour 61, 1013–1021.
CrossRef |

Bates, D., Maechler, M., and Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-42. Available at: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4

Bell, A. M. (2007). Future directions in behavioural syndromes research. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 274, 755–761.
CrossRef |

Bergvall, U. A., Schäpers, A., Kjellander, P., and Weiss, A. (2011). Personality and foraging decisions in fallow deer (Dama dama). Animal Behaviour 81, 101–112.
CrossRef |

Best, E. C. (2013). Network analysis of the social structure of female eastern grey kangaroos (Macropus giganteus). Ph.D. Thesis. Univeristy of Queensland, Brisbane.

Blumstein, D. T., Runyan, A., Seymour, M., Nicodemus, A., Ozgul, A., Ransler, F., Im, S., Stark, T., Zugmeyer, C., and Daniel, J. C. (2004). Locomotor ability and wariness in yellow-bellied marmots. Journal of Ethology 110, 615–634.
CrossRef |

Brook, L. A., Johnson, C. N., and Ritchie, E. G. (2012). Effects of predator control on behaviour of an apex predator and indirect consequences for mesopredator suppression. Journal of Applied Ecology 49, 1278–1286.
CrossRef |

Brown, J. S. (1999). Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Evolutionary Ecology Research 1, 49–71.

Burger, J., and Gochfeld, M. (1994). Vigilance in African mammals – differences among mothers, other females, and males. Behaviour 131, 153–169.
CrossRef |

Cameron, E. Z., and du Toit, J. T. (2005). Social influences on vigilance behaviour in giraffes, Giraffa camelopardalis. Animal Behaviour 69, 1337–1344.
CrossRef |

Carter, K., and Goldizen, A. W. (2003). Habitat choice and vigilance behaviour of brush-tailed rock-wallabies (Petrogale penicillata) within their nocturnal foraging ranges. Wildlife Research 30, 355–364.
CrossRef |

Carter, A. J., Macdonald, S. L., Thomson, V. A., and Goldizen, A. W. (2009a). Structured association patterns and their energetic benefits in female eastern grey kangaroos, Macropus giganteus. Animal Behaviour 77, 839–846.
CrossRef |

Carter, A. J., Pays, O., and Goldizen, A. W. (2009b). Individual variation in the relationship between vigilance and group size in eastern grey kangaroos. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64, 237–245.
CrossRef |

Carter, A. J., Goldizen, A. W., and Tromp, S. A. (2010). Agamas exhibit behavioural syndromes: bolder males bask and feed more but may suffer higher predation. Behavioral Ecology 21, 655–661.
CrossRef |

Childress, M. J., and Lung, M. A. (2003). Predation risk, gender and the group size effect: does elk vigilance depend upon the behaviour of conspecifics? Animal Behaviour 66, 389–398.
CrossRef |

Clarke, J. L., Jones, M. E., and Jarman, P. J. (1995). Diurnal and nocturnal grouping and foraging behaviours of free-ranging eastern grey kangaroos. Australian Journal of Zoology 43, 519–529.
CrossRef |

Colagross, A. M. L., and Cockburn, A. (1993). Vigilance and grouping in the eastern gray kangaroo, Macropus giganteus. Australian Journal of Zoology 41, 325–334.
CrossRef |

Cork, S. J. (1991). Meeting the energy requirements for lactation in a macropodid marsupial: current nutrition versus stored body reserves. Journal of Zoology 225, 567–576.
CrossRef |

Dannock, R. J., Blomberg, S. P., and Goldizen, A. W. (2013). Individual variation in vigilance in female eastern grey kangaroos. Australian Journal of Zoology , .

Dewitt, T. J., Sih, A., and Hucko, J. A. (1999). Trait compensation and cospecialization in a freshwater snail: size, shape and antipredator behaviour. Animal Behaviour 58, 397–407.
CrossRef | PubMed |

Dingemanse, N. J., Dochtermann, N. A., and Nakagawa, S. (2012). Defining behavioural syndromes and the role of ‘syndrome deviation’ in explaining their evolution. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66, 1543–1548.
CrossRef |

Dyer, J. R. G., Croft, D. P., Morrell, L. J., and Krause, J. (2009). Shoal composition determines foraging success in the guppy. Behavioral Ecology 20, 165–171.
CrossRef |

Elgar, M. A. (1989). Predator vigilance and group-size in mammals and birds – a critical review of the empirical evidence. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 64, 13–33.
CrossRef | CAS | PubMed |

Fairbanks, B., and Dobson, F. S. (2007). Mechanisms of the group-size effect on vigilance in Columbian ground squirrels: dilution versus detection. Animal Behaviour 73, 115–123.
CrossRef |

Favreau, F. R., Goldizen, A. W., and Pays, O. (2010). Interactions among social monitoring, anti-predator vigilance and group size in eastern grey kangaroos. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 277, 2089–2095.
CrossRef |

Garamszegi, L. Z., and Herczeg, G. (2012). Behavioural syndromes, syndrome deviation and the within- and between-individual components of phenotypic correlations: when reality does not meet statistics. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66, 1651–1658.
CrossRef |

Gaynor, K. M., and Cords, M. (2012). Antipredator and social monitoring functions of vigilance behaviour in blue monkeys. Animal Behaviour 84, 531–537.
CrossRef |

Harden, R. H. (1985). The ecology of the dingo in north-eastern New South Wales. 1. Movements and home range. Australian Wildlife Research 12, 25–37.
CrossRef |

Heathcote, C. F. (1987). Grouping of eastern grey kangaroos in open habitat. Australian Wildlife Research 14, 343–348.
CrossRef |

Heptner, V., and Naumov, N. (1998). Sirenia and Carnivora (seacows, bears and wolves). In ‘Mammals of the Soviet Union. Vol. II, Part 1a’. (Science Publishers, Inc.: USA.)

Hoogland, J. L., Hale, S. L., Kirk, A. D., and Sui, Y. D. (2013). Individual variation in vigilance among white-tailed prairie dogs (Cynomys leucurus). The Southwestern Naturalist , .

Houston, A. I., McNamara, J. M., and Hutchinson, J. M. C. (1993). General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 341, 375–397.
CrossRef |

Hunter, L. T. B., and Skinner, J. D. (1998). Vigilance behaviour in African ungulates: the role of predation pressure. Behaviour 135, 195–211.
CrossRef |

Jarman, P. J. (1987). Group-size and activity in eastern grey kangaroos. Animal Behaviour 35, 1044–1050.
CrossRef |

Jarman, P. J. (1994). Individual behaviour and social organisation of kangaroos. In ‘Animal Societies: Individuals, Interactions and Organisation’. (Eds P. J. Jarman, and A. Rossiter.) pp. 69–85. (Kyoto University Press: Kyoto.)

Jarman, P. J., Jones, M. E., Johnson, C. N., Southwell, C. J., Stuartdick, R. I., Higginbottom, K. B., and Clarke, J. L. (1989). Macropod studies at Wallaby Creek. 8. Individual recognition of kangaroos and wallabies. Australian Wildlife Research 16, 179–185.
CrossRef |

Kurvers, R. H. J. M., Prins, H. H. T., van Wieren, S. E., van Oers, K., Nolet, B. A., and Ydenberg, R. C. (2010). The effect of personality on social foraging: shy barnacle geese scrounge more. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 277, 601–608.
CrossRef |

Lea, A. J., and Blumstein, D. T. (2011). Age and sex influence marmot antipredator behaviour during periods of heightened risk. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65, 1525–1533.
CrossRef | PubMed |

Lima, S. L. (1995). Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance – the group-size effect. Animal Behaviour 49, 11–20.
CrossRef |

Lung, M. A., and Childress, M. J. (2007). The influence of conspecifics and predation risk on the vigilance of elk (Cervus elaphus) in Yellowstone National Park. Behavioral Ecology 18, 12–20.
CrossRef |

Monclús, R., and Rödel, H. G. (2009). Influence of different individual traits on vigilance behaviour in European rabbits. Journal of Ethology 115, 758–766.
CrossRef |

Pangle, W. M., and Holekamp, K. E. (2010). Functions of vigilance behaviour in a social carnivore, the spotted hyaena, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour 80, 257–267.
CrossRef |

Patrick, S. C., Chapman, J. R., Dugdale, H. L., Quinn, J. L., and Sheldon, B. C. (2012). Promiscuity, paternity and personality in the great tit. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 279, 1724–1730.
CrossRef |

Pays, O., and Jarman, P. J. (2008). Does sex affect both individual and collective vigilance in social mammalian herbivores: the case of the eastern grey kangaroo? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62, 757–767.
CrossRef |

Pays, O., Goulard, M., Blomberg, S. P., Goldizen, A. W., Sirot, E., and Jarman, P. J. (2009). The effect of social facilitation on vigilance in the eastern gray kangaroo, Macropus giganteus. Behavioral Ecology 20, 469–477.
CrossRef |

Pinheiro, J. C., and Bates, D. M. (2000). ‘Mixed-effects Models in S and S-PLUS.’ (Springer.)

Preisser, E. L., Bolnick, D. I., and Benard, M. F. (2005). Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator–prey interactions. Ecology 86, 501–509.
CrossRef |

Pulliam, H. R. (1973). Advantages in flocking. Journal of Theoretical Biology 38, 419–422.
CrossRef | CAS | PubMed |

R Core Team (2012). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: http://www.R-project.org

Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., and Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 82, 291–318.
CrossRef | PubMed |

Rieucau, G., and Martin, J. G. A. (2008). Many eyes or many ewes: vigilance tactics in female bighorn sheep Ovis canadensis vary according to reproductive status. Oikos 117, 501–506.
CrossRef |

Rieucau, G., Morand-Ferron, J., and Giraldeau, L.-A. (2010). Group size effect in nutmeg mannikin: between-individuals behavioural differences but same plasticity. Behavioral Ecology 21, 684–689.
CrossRef |

Roberts, G. (1996). Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. Animal Behaviour 51, 1077–1086.
CrossRef |

Shorrocks, B., and Cockayne, A. (2005). Vigilance and group size in impala (Aepyceros melampus Lichtenstein): a study in Nairobi National Park, Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 43, 91–96.
CrossRef |

Sih, A., Bell, A., and Johnson, J. C. (2004). Behavioural syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19, 372–378.
CrossRef |

Smith, A. C., Kelez, S., and Buchanan-Smith, H. M. (2004). Factors affecting vigilance within wild mixed-species troops of saddleback (Saguinus fuscicollis) and moustached tamarins (S. mystax). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56, 18–25.
CrossRef |

Southwell, C. J. (1984a). Variability in grouping in the eastern grey kangaroo, Macropus giganteus. 1. Group density and group-size. Australian Wildlife Research 11, 423–435.
CrossRef |

Southwell, C. J. (1984b). Variability in grouping in the eastern grey kangaroo, Macropus giganteus. 2. Dynamics of group formation. Australian Wildlife Research 11, 437–449.
CrossRef |

Stankowich, T., and Blumstein, D. T. (2005). Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 272, 2627–2634.
CrossRef |

Steenbeek, R., Piek, R. C., van Buul, M., and van Hooff, J. (1999). Vigilance in wild Thomas’s langurs (Presbytis thomasi): the importance of infanticide risk. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 45, 137–150.
CrossRef |

Treves, A., Drescher, A., and Snowdon, C. T. (2003). Maternal watchfulness in black howler monkeys (Alouatta pigra). Journal of Ethology 109, 135–146.
CrossRef |

Unck, C. E., Waterman, J. M., Verburgt, L., and Bateman, P. W. (2009). Quantity versus quality: how does level of predation threat affect Cape ground squirrel vigilance? Animal Behaviour 78, 625–632.
CrossRef |

Valeix, M., Fritz, H., Loveridge, A. J., Davidson, Z., Hunt, J. E., Murindagomo, F., and Macdonald, D. W. (2009). Does the risk of encountering lions influence African herbivore behaviour at waterholes? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63, 1483–1494.
CrossRef |

Wilson, A. D. M., and Godin, J.-G. J. (2010). Boldness and intermittent locomotion in the bluegill sunfish, Lepomis macrochirus. Behavioral Ecology 21, 57–62.
CrossRef |

Wilson, A. J., de Boer, M., Arnott, G., and Grimmer, A. (2011). Integrating personality research and animal contest theory: aggressiveness in the green swordtail Xiphophorus helleri. PLoS ONE 6, e28024.
CrossRef | CAS | PubMed |

Wolf, M., van Doorn, G. S., Leimar, O., and Weissing, F. J. (2007). Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal personalities. Nature 447, 581–584.
CrossRef | CAS | PubMed |

Wright, J., Maklakov, A. A., and Khazin, V. (2001). State-dependent sentinels: an experimental study in the Arabian babbler. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 268, 821–826.
CrossRef |

Yeo, I. K., and Johnson, R. A. (2000). A new family of power transformations to improve normality or symmetry. Biometrika 87, 954–959.
CrossRef |


   
Subscriber Login
Username:
Password:  

 
    
Legal & Privacy | Contact Us | Help

CSIRO

© CSIRO 1996-2014