Register      Login
International Journal of Wildland Fire International Journal of Wildland Fire Society
Journal of the International Association of Wildland Fire
RESEARCH FRONT

Children’s knowledge of bushfire emergency response

Briony Towers
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

Geospatial and Mathematical Sciences, SET Portfolio, RMIT, GPO Box 2476V, Melbourne, Vic. 3001, Australia. Email: briony.towers@rmit.edu.au

International Journal of Wildland Fire 24(2) 179-189 https://doi.org/10.1071/WF13153
Submitted: 11 September 2013  Accepted: 5 January 2015   Published: 10 March 2015

Abstract

In the aftermath of the Black Saturday bushfire disaster, the Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission recommended that bushfire education be made a formal part of the Australian national curriculum. Crucially, the success of any hazards education program depends on the degree to which the learner’s existing knowledge and experience is accommodated in the education process. Yet accommodating children’s knowledge in bushfire education is hampered by a lack of research on how children conceptualise bushfire hazards. To address this gap, this paper presents a detailed qualitative analysis of children’s knowledge of bushfire hazards with a specific focus on emergency response. Across four bushfire-prone locations in south-eastern Australia, 26 focus group interviews were conducted with 87 children aged between 8 and 12 years. To better understand the construction of children’s knowledge, individual interviews were also conducted with 37 parents. Through their engagement in the research process, children demonstrated a capacity for engaging in serious discussions about bushfire hazards and although their knowledge was often characterised by gaps and misconceptions, they also demonstrated a capacity for understanding the fundamental principles of emergency response, particularly when they had been involved in bushfire planning within their household.

Additional keywords: emergency management, hazards education, wildfire.

Introduction

On 7 February 2009, bushfires (wildfires) burned 450 000 ha of Victorian bushland, killing 173 people (including 27 children) and destroying over 2000 homes (Teague et al. 2010). In the immediate aftermath of what is now referred to as ‘Black Saturday’, the Victorian Government established the Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission, which attributed the magnitude of losses to a fundamental lack of bushfire knowledge and awareness within the affected communities (Teague et al. 2010). Identifying bushfire education for children as the most effective means of addressing this issue, the Commission’s final report included a formal recommendation for bushfire education be integrated in the Australian national curriculum (Teague et al. 2010).

However, the success of any hazards education depends heavily on the degree to which the learner’s existing knowledge is accommodated in the education process (Twigg 2004). For several decades, scholars have argued that when hazard education is designed without regard for how different groups understand and experience hazards, it fails to achieve the kind of learning that facilitates protective action (Perry and Mushkatel 1986; Handmer and Penning-Rowsell 1990; Twigg 2004; Haynes et al. 2008). Respecting and accommodating the knowledge of the learner is also the central tenet of several prominent psychological theories that emphasise the influence of existing conceptual networks on the interpretation of new information and novel situations (Donaldson 1978; Von Glasersfeld 1991; Bruner 1996; Piercey and Berlyne 2001; Kushnir and Xu 2012).

Yet accommodating children’s existing knowledge in bushfire education is severely hampered by a distinct lack of research on how children conceptualise bushfire hazards. In recognition of this research gap, this paper presents a detailed qualitative analysis of children’s knowledge of bushfire hazards with a specific focus on emergency response. The paper begins with a review of the existing literature on children’s knowledge of hazards and disasters and the Australian approach to community bushfire safety. It then outlines the Grounded Theory methodology and child-centred research methods that were employed to gather and analyse the data. Next, it presents key findings and discusses their implications for children’s bushfire education. The paper concludes with a call for increased child participation in emergency bushfire planning both at school and within their households.


Children’s knowledge of hazards and disasters

Historically, children have been largely excluded from the international hazards and disasters research agenda (Anderson 2005; Peek 2008). Over the last several years, however, there has been a substantial increase in child-centred research outputs (cf. American Bar Association 2009; Kilmer et al. 2010; Shaw et al. 2012). Although a substantial proportion of this research has focussed on children’s psychosocial adjustment in the aftermath of disaster (Masten and Narayan 2012; Pfefferbaum et al. 2013), a small but growing body of research has focussed specifically on children’s knowledge and education (Ronan and Johnston 2005; Mitchell et al. 2008; Finnis et al. 2010; Haynes et al. 2010b; Ronan et al. 2010; Shaw et al. 2011; Ronan and Towers 2014).

In the most empirically rigorous studies to date, Ronan and colleagues (Ronan and Johnston 2001; Ronan et al. 2001; Ronan and Johnston 2003; Finnis et al. 2004; Finnis et al. 2010; Ronan et al. 2010) used quantitative questionnaire-based methods to investigate the influence of school-based hazards education on a range of variables, including children’s knowledge of protective response behaviours for various hazards (i.e. floods, storms, structural fires, earthquakes, snow storms and tsunami). For each hazard, 5- to 12-year-old children were presented with a list of correct and incorrect protective response behaviours and were asked to indicate which behaviours they would endorse in the event of that hazard. Across studies, children who had participated in school-based hazards education endorsed an increased number of correct behaviours and a decreased number of incorrect behaviours. Additionally, more accurate knowledge of response behaviours was associated with a reduction in hazard-related fears, which is consistent with the wider literature on childhood fear and anxiety (Gullone 2000; Klein 2009).

Although the New Zealand-based studies highlight the benefits of school-based hazards education, the exclusive use of quantitative questionnaire-based methods warrants some discussion. In recent years, it has been argued that quantitative methods restrict the researcher’s ability to capture a detailed understanding of children’s knowledge and experience because a priori theories and generalisations of adults are imposed on the data (Woodgate 2000a, 2000b; James and Prout 2004; Corsaro 2005; Greig et al. 2007). Yet it can never be assumed that children’s knowledge will resemble that of adults. Indeed, child-centred research outputs have demonstrated that discrepancies often exist between the personal meanings that children and adults attribute to actions and events (Greig et al. 2007). Thus, inductive, qualitative research that enables the emergence of children’s own perspectives on hazards and disasters is needed.


Community bushfire safety in Australia

The general philosophy underlying the Australian approach to community bushfire safety is that those at risk should play an active role in managing and reducing that risk (Handmer and Haynes 2008). For many years, the centrepiece of the community safety approach has been the ‘prepare, stay and defend or leave early’ policy (PSDLE) (Tibbits et al. 2008; Australasian Fire and Emergency Service Authorities Council (AFAC) 2012). Under this policy, residents are advised to either prepare to stay and defend their homes and properties, or prepare and leave well before a fire arrives in their area (AFAC 2012). The policy is based on substantial empirical evidence that (i) people are more likely to be killed by radiant heat or a vehicle accident while evacuating, and (ii) well-prepared houses can be successfully defended and provide safe refuge for people during the main passage of the fire front (Handmer and Tibbits 2005; Tibbits et al. 2008). Although these assumptions were scrutinised in the wake of Black Saturday, they were reinforced by the evidence presented to the Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission and the policy was retained (Handmer et al. 2010; Teague et al. 2010; Whittaker et al. 2013).

Although the PSDLE policy is empirically robust, an expanding body of research has identified fundamental problems with implementation (Tibbits and Whittaker 2007; Whittaker et al. 2013). Despite fire agencies’ concerted efforts to educate the community, many residents living in high-risk areas eschew the binary approach of PSDLE in favour of dangerous ‘wait and see’ strategies (Whittaker et al. 2013). Numerous studies have shown that residents who plan to leave often wait until the fire has reached the vicinity of their property, while residents who plan to stay defend often retain late evacuation as a last-minute option (Tibbits and Whittaker 2007; Whittaker et al. 2013). To address this issue, Australian fire agencies have introduced a new ‘Code Red’ fire danger rating that is applied when the Forest Fire Danger Index exceeds 100 (Bureau of Meteorology 2014). On Code Red days, residents are advised to leave either the day before or early in the morning, even if they are well prepared to stay and defend (AFAC 2012). Yet a recent study of household responses to a Code Red rating found that very few residents left their homes in the absence of an actual fire threat (Whittaker and Handmer 2010).

Despite their well-documented vulnerability to bushfire disasters (McFarlane 1987; McDermott et al. 2005; Haynes et al. 2010a; Yelland et al. 2010), Australian children have been marginalised in both academic and political discussions of bushfire community safety. To date, no research has investigated how children understand bushfire emergency response and the only official advice relating to children is that they should not be present during a bushfire event (AFAC 2012). Although well-intentioned, this advice does not provide children or their families with the information they need to critically engage with bushfire planning. Nor does it reflect the reality that even under Code Red conditions, many people will continue to ‘wait and see’ (Whittaker and Handmer 2010). Thus, there is a need for evidence-based education programs that provide children with the requisite knowledge and skills for responding safely to bushfire emergencies. Understanding children’s knowledge of bushfire response represents a crucial first step in that direction.


Research strategy

Grounded Theory methodology

Given the dearth of child-centred research in this area, this research employed a Grounded Theory (GT) methodology (Glaser and Strauss 1967; Charmaz 2006; Strauss and Corbin 1998, 2008). Grounded Theory is an inductive methodology for developing new theory that is rigorously ‘grounded’ in data (Charmaz 2006; Strauss and Corbin 2008). The rigour inherent in GT can be largely attributed to the three phases of coding: (1) initial coding – naming each word, line, or segment of the data; (2) focussed coding – sorting the most frequent initial codes into broader categories; and (3) theoretical coding – weaving the categories together to create a cohesive account of the studied phenomenon (Charmaz 2006). Throughout each coding phase, the method of constant comparison – comparing data with data, codes with codes, and categories with categories – further ensures a high level of analytic rigour (Charmaz 2006). Additionally, the assumptions and techniques of GT are consistent with contemporary theories of child development, making it an ideal methodology for researching children’s knowledge (Woodgate 2000a, 2000b; Coyne 2006; Sartain et al. 2008).

Qualitative methods

Although qualitative methods have not traditionally been viewed as an appropriate choice for research with children, the social sciences are now more accepting of children as reliable informants who are more than capable of providing accurate accounts of their own knowledge and experience (Qvortrup 1994; Curtin 2001; James and Prout 2004; Greig et al. 2007). At the same time, conducting qualitative research with children poses some unique challenges. One issue pertains to children’s still-developing communication abilities (Greig et al. 2007). Another pertains to the power differential that exists between child participants and adult researchers (Eder and Fingerson 2002). In the present research, children’s communication abilities were augmented through the use of the ‘draw and write’ technique, in which children draw a scenario and then write or talk about what is happening in their picture (Pridmore and Lansdown 1997; Horstman et al. 2008). Meanwhile, the power differential was reduced through the use of focus group interviews, which are said to increase children’s confidence in expressing their viewpoints, presumably because children are more at ease when they are among their peers and outnumber the adults in the setting (Eder and Fingerson 2002; Hennessey and Heary 2005).

Data collection and analysis

Focus group interviews with children were conducted in two primary schools in Tasmania (Bothwell District School and Huonville Primary School) and two primary schools in Victoria (Macedon Primary School and Warrandyte Primary School). The schools were selected on the basis of two main criteria: (1) the location of the school was identified by the relevant state fire authorities as one that could be impacted by a bushfire event; and (2) there had not been a major bushfire in that location in the lifetimes of the children living there. The main reason underlying this second criterion was that a major bushfire in any particular location is a relatively infrequent occurrence. Thus, education programs must be informed by analyses of how children understand bushfire emergency response in the absence of any direct experience with a major bushfire event.

On agreeing to participate in the research, school principals were contacted and times for classroom recruitment presentations were arranged. Across the four schools, 87 children aged between 8 and 12 years participated in 26 focus group interviews. Male to female ratios approached 1 : 1 with 40 males (46%) and 47 females (54%). To gain additional insight into the construction of children’s knowledge, 37 individual interviews were conducted with parents of the child participants. Parents’ ages ranged from 32 to 47 years with an average age of 41 years. Females were disproportionately represented with 31 females (81.1%) and 6 males (18.9%). This is explained by the fact that many of the female participants were either taking leave from work or working part-time in order to raise their young families and this made it easier for them to participate.

Tasmanian focus groups and parent interviews were conducted during the months of September, October and November in the lead-up to the 2007–08 bushfire season. Victorian focus groups and parent interviews were conducted exactly 1 year later, during the months of September, October and November in the lead up to the 2008–09 bushfire season. Thus, all data collection was completed prior to Black Saturday. The number of participants in each focus group ranged from three to five children, which is considered an ideal size for maximising discussion, yet still maintaining order (Peek and Fothergill 2009).

Focus groups with children were structured around a scaffolded scenario that commenced with receiving a warning about a bushfire in the area and concluded with a fire moving over the property.1 All children were given the option of using the ‘draw and write’ technique to assist them in communicating their thoughts and ideas. In the parent interviews, participants were asked to describe their household bushfire plan and the extent to which their children had participated in household bushfire planning. Focus groups were video-recorded and parent interviews were audio-recorded. All recordings were transcribed verbatim and transcripts were analysed using the GT coding procedures described above (Charmaz 2006).

The research was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee (Tasmania) Network, which adopts the guidelines of the National Health and Medical Research Council on Ethical Conduct in Research (National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) 2014). Approval to conduct the research in schools was granted by the Victorian Department of Education and Training and the Tasmanian Department of Education.


Research findings

Through their engagement in the research process, the children demonstrated a distinct capacity for engaging in serious discussions about bushfire hazards. They were highly aware that there could be bushfire in their area and they clearly understood the importance of planning for bushfire emergencies: “People need to have plans. Before there’s a fire, they need to have plans” (Taylor, 9 years, Macedon). Consistent with previous research (e.g. Finnis et al. 2010; Ronan et al. 2010), the children also suggested that knowing how to respond to a bushfire emergency helped to assuage their bushfire-related fears. For example, when asked if he was worried about bushfires in his area, Jared (8 years, Macedon) responded, “No, I’m not because our family’s got a fire plan and stuff”. Similarly, Michaela (11 years, Macedon) explained that “I want to know what I would have to do because then I would feel a bit more relaxed.”

In the focus group interviews, children identified three main approaches to bushfire emergency response: leaving, staying to defend and sheltering. A detailed exposition of children’s knowledge of each approach is set out below. The quotes presented have been selected because they most clearly represent children’s knowledge of the concept or phenomenon in question. However, a more extensive selection of quotes can be found in Towers (2012).

Leaving

Children’s knowledge of leaving was structured around three key tasks: deciding what to take, choosing a destination, and identifying triggers.

The task of deciding what to take sprang from children’s awareness that if they did leave, their home might be destroyed in their absence. When deciding what to take, children listed a wide variety of special belongings including toys, photographs, jewellery and other family heirlooms: for most children, however, it was pets that took first priority (Table 1).


Table 1.  Deciding what to take
Click to zoom

Although most children planned to gather their special belongings when faced with an actual bushfire threat, some children had packed a ‘firebox’ in anticipation of a future event:

Dell: We’ve got a firebox in case we have to just get away. You get a cardboard box and put all your really valuable stuff in it, in case your house is burnt. (10 years, Bothwell)

The task of choosing a destination involved identifying a location that was perceived as being ‘away’ from any potential fire threat. Some children chose a destination in another town or city, where the home of friends or relatives would provide a safe refuge until the fire threat had passed.

Pete: The safest place for us to go would be to hop into our car and drive off to my mum’s best friend’s house up near NSW, about 5 minutes to the end of Victoria. It’s in Kerang. So we would go up there and that’s the safest place. (9 years, Warrandyte)

Other children, meanwhile, chose destinations much closer to home, such as the mailbox, the end of the driveway or a neighbour’s house (Table 2).


Table 2.  Choosing destinations close to home
Click to zoom

The tendency to choose destinations so close to home seemed to be an artefact of school-based house-fire safety programs, which routinely include a homework activity whereby children develop a house-fire escape plan with their family (Country Fire Authority (CFA) 2014; TasFire 2014). Many of the parents interviewed explicitly acknowledged that it was this homework activity that had compelled them to develop a detailed plan:

Melissa: The children came home with an assignment where we had to work out a house [fire] plan. They took it really seriously so we sat down with them and worked out exactly what we would do, what our exit points would be and where we would meet outside. I couldn’t believe we hadn’t already done it: something so easy that could save our lives. (Mother of Fiona (9 years) and Colin (11 years), Huonville)

Crucially, however, when asked about their household bushfire plans, children often recited the details of what were undoubtedly house-fire escape plans:

Researcher: How many of you have a bushfire emergency plan with your family?

Sarah: We’ve got our meeting place, we do.

Kevin: Mum said that, like in my old house you’d knock over the like flyscreen, jump out that, then go round the paddock and meet at the front gate. (11 years, Huonville)

Moreover, when specifically asked if their house fire plans could be implemented in a bushfire emergency, some children responded in the affirmative:

Cate: Well, if when we see a fire, we all run out to the letterbox and mum says is everybody here and like there’s only four people, um, then she goes back in and finds them if they’re still asleep.

Researcher: And are those plans for a bushfire or a house fire, do you think?

Cate: Both. (9 years, Warrandyte)

Ostensibly, children did not readily differentiate between house-fires and bushfires and, in the absence of any information to the contrary, they had assumed that their house-fire plan could be suitably applied to the bushfire context.

Identifying triggers involved deciding on the precise point at which the family should leave. Some children said they would leave as soon as they received a warning, even if there was no imminent threat.

Researcher: What would you do if there was a fire on the mountain?

Max: We’d probably leave to go to New Gisborne because fire is very hot.

Researcher: So you’d leave even if the fire was still up there?

Max: Yes. Well, just because my parents would probably not want to even take any chances. (11 years, Macedon)

Other children, by contrast, advocated a ‘wait and see’ strategy similar to that which has been identified in research with adults (Tibbits and Whittaker 2007; Whittaker et al. 2013). These children suggested that they would pack up their valuables and pets, but would only leave when the fire reached the vicinity of their property.

Researcher: What if the fire was still over up in the hills, what would you do then?

Brendan: Just leave it for a while.

Mike: Just stay. Stay and see what happens.

Researcher: While you were waiting, what would you be doing?

Brendan: Just watching the fire to see how close it gets.

Mandy: Get most of your stuff so you can go when it comes like really close to you. (10 years, Bothwell)

Children advocating this ‘wait and see’ strategy lacked an awareness of the dangers of late evacuation. For example, when asked what they would do if the fire was blocking their evacuation route, some children suggested they would go ‘around’ or ‘through’ the fire, reflecting a lack of knowledge about the intensity and magnitude of both smoke and radiant heat.

Claire: If the fire went up the hill, it could go in five directions: it could go there, there, there, and there and there, so it could straight to my house. But if it goes to my house, I could just go around it. (9 years, Warrandyte)

Thomas: You could probably just drive straight through the fire. It would probably only, like, peel the paint off the car and you could just drive a bit faster through the fire. (11 years, Bothwell)

Other children suggested fleeing in the opposite direction, reflecting a lack of knowledge about the speed at which bushfires can travel.

Claire: I would go around the pond but if there was a fire there, I would have to run the other way down the driveway. (11 years, Huonville)

Importantly, however, some children did understand the dangers of late evacuation and advised against trying to leave if the fire was too close.

Max: If like the fire was fairly close, you wouldn’t really go on the road, because that’s actually how a lot people died in Ash Wednesday; they tried to get out of their house and run away. (11 years, Macedon)

Some of these children also suggested that if it was too late to leave, they would initiate a contingency plan to stay and defend, which is consistent with the advice of Australian fire agencies (AFAC 2012).

Philip: If we’re going to go, we’ll take everything in the car but, if the fire’s too close, we’ll get everything into our front room, which is made of brick, and we thought we’d just get like wet mops and stuff to put out the fire if it gets too close. (12 years, Macedon)

As revealed in focus groups and interviews with parents, the accuracy and sophistication of children’s knowledge of leaving was largely determined by their participation in household emergency planning. When children had been involved in planning activities and discussions, they tended to advocate leaving early to a destination that was far removed from the area under threat. For example, Stuart (11 years, Macedon) reported that “If there’s a fire in the area, if we weren’t at school, me and my brother and mum would go to Highpoint [suburban shopping centre] or something and my dad stays and defends the house”. As Stuart’s mother explained, she had tried to ensure that her children would know what to expect if they did need to leave:

Jan: Coming from the suburbs, we were really aware [of the risk] from the beginning, from the very day we moved in and we’ve tried to make that part of the kids’ understanding as well. The children know that I’ll take them and we’ll go somewhere safe. You know, we’ll make that decision early and we’ll go somewhere safe. (Mother of Stuart (11 years), Macedon)

By contrast, when children had not been involved in household bushfire planning, they often chose a destination close to the house or advocated dangerous ‘wait and see’ strategies. For example, Debbie’s mother admitted that she was ‘not really comfortable with our plan because I don’t think the kids are aware of what’s needed and what would be required’. Indeed, Debbie’s plan was to evacuate to her grandmother’s house, but only when the fire had reached the vicinity of her property:

Researcher: If there is a bushfire that is a threat to your area, what do you do?

Debbie: We would get in the car and probably go to my grandma’s house or something.

Researcher: Alright and under what circumstances do you get in the car?

Debbie: If like you could see everything and it was like really close, then we’d probably go. (11 years, Macedon)

Staying to defend

Children’s knowledge of staying to defend was structured around three distinct phases of a bushfire event: before the fire, as the fire approaches, and when the fire arrives. For each phase, children proposed various tasks and activities that were aimed at protecting life and property.

In addition to general property maintenance (e.g. removing trees, raking leaves, mowing lawns, cleaning gutters), tasks to be undertaken before the fire included ensuring a dedicated water supply for firefighting and obtaining firefighting equipment (Table 3). As will be noted, many children had observed these activities taking place on their own properties.


Table 3.  Tasks to be undertaken before the fire arrives
Click to zoom

Tasks to be undertaken as the fire approaches included setting up the firefighting equipment, preparing additional water resources, and taking additional precautions to prevent house ignition (Table 4).


Table 4.  Tasks to be undertaken as the fire approaches
Click to zoom

Although children’s knowledge of what to do before a fire arrives and as a fire approaches was generally consistent with the advice of Australian fire agencies (AFAC 2012), their knowledge of how to respond when the fire arrives was often misconceived. Many children suggested that they would fight the fire front itself with the explicit aim of preventing the fire from reaching the house. Moreover, if this proved unsuccessful, they would initiate a last-minute evacuation.

Con: Well, because our house is sort of protected, we’d try and start to put out this fire, yeah, the main fire because like we’ll have spray packs on and they’ll be full of water and because I sort of know how to drive a little car that we have and it’s got a big water tank on there, so we can use that as well to try and put that fire out.

Researcher: What if the fire kept coming?

Con: If it gets really, really close, you have to evacuate. (9 years, Macedon)

These children not only lacked lack an awareness of the dangers of late evacuation, they also had misconceptions about the causes of house ignition. Although extensive research has shown that house ignition is predominantly caused by ember attack in the periods before and after the main fire front passes over (Leonard 2003; Blanchi and Leonard 2008), these children were adamant that if the fire front did pass over, the house would burn, regardless of the precautions that been taken to protect it.

Researcher: What will happen if a bushfire comes right up near the house?

Con: It’s probably still gonna burn. If it gets really close, they have to hop in their car and drive away.

Researcher: But why wouldn’t you stay if you’ve done all that work to get the house ready for bushfire season?

Con: If it gets too close you have to leave.

Researcher: So if the fire comes up close…

Con: Evacuate.

Researcher: Nothing is going to save the house?

Larry: No. Absolutely not. (9 years, Macedon)

However, there were some children whose approach to defending was highly consistent with the advice of Australian fire agencies (AFAC 2012): specifically, patrolling the property extinguishing embers and spot fires, sheltering from radiant heat inside the house as the fire front passed over, and then returning outside to extinguish any remaining spot fires after the fire front had passed.

Brian: If it’s [the fire] around the house, then you start defending. You just start putting out the embers that fall near the house and if there’s any spot fires you put them out.

Researcher: What would you do when the main fire was from here to the playground away [10 m]?

Brian: You’d go inside and you’d just wait until the fire front has passed… Because of the radiant heat, if you’re still trying to put out the spot fires and everything, the heat will get to you… You’d stay in the middle of the house like as far away from the windows as possible and you move all the furniture into the middle and you’d wait until the fire front has passed and then you’d go back out and keep defending. (9 years, Macedon)

Although these children did not expect their homes to catch fire, they did propose contingency plans to be implemented in the event that a fire did take hold:

Brian: I have a backup plan, if your house starts burning and this might not happen, but like if you’ve got like an island in the middle of your dam, you could go to your island. Because it’s going to be really hot and it will be really low so you could almost walk there or you could swim, it’s not going to be very far. (9 years, Macedon)

The sophistication of children’s knowledge of staying to defend was also largely explained by the extent to which they had participated in household emergency planning. For example, Brian (9 years, Macedon), whose perspectives are presented above, had been fully briefed on the intricacies of his household’s plan to stay and defend, including how to respond when the fire arrived:

Sally: I think that the more information he had and the more I could explain, you know like, ‘The fire front’s only this amount of time and while it’s happening we’re inside we’re safe, the rest of the time you’re patrolling, you’re putting out embers, it’s understandable, it’s manageable, the fire doesn’t just land on your house and blow it up’. (Mother of Brian (8 years), Warrandyte)

By contrast, when children had been excluded from discussions about staying to defend, they were more likely to advocate last-minute evacuations. For example, Jane (9 years, Warrandyte), who planned to fight the fire front and then run down to the river when it reached the house, had been excluded from family discussions on the basis of her age:

Bob: I suppose we’ve probably tended to shield her a bit from it up to an extent. Maybe because she’s just turning 9 this year, so it’s probably that up until now we’ve thought she didn’t really need to know about that to an extent… it’s probably something that we need to think about. What is a good age to talk about it and that might be maybe 11 or 12. (Father of Jane (9 years), Warrandyte)

Sheltering

Unlike staying to defend, which involved active firefighting activities, sheltering involved taking passive refuge in a structure or location deemed to be ‘fireproof’. Children’s preferred structures and locations for sheltering fell into three main categories: buildings, bodies of water and underground bunkers (Table 5).


Table 5.  Structures and locations for sheltering
Click to zoom

To consider buildings to be safe places in which to shelter, children had to be certain that they would fully resist bushfire impacts. Construction materials were considered to be the key predictor of resistance, and metal, brick and stone all ranked highly in children’s assessments. Although construction materials do influence the likelihood of building ignition, other variables, such as structure design, preparedness of the property and human activities during the fire, also play an integral role (Blanchi and Leonard 2008; Blanchi et al. 2010). The critical role of human activity during the fire has been highlighted in two key studies of Australian bushfire fatalities (Handmer et al. 2010; Haynes et al. 2010a), in which over one-third of all bushfire fatalities were attributed to passive sheltering inside. For this reason, Australian fire agencies discourage passive sheltering in any building that is not a formally designated community shelter or bushfire refuge (AFAC 2012).

Children often perceived bodies of water (e.g. dams, swimming pools, rivers and bathtubs) as safe shelters because they believed that the water would act as a non-flammable protective barrier between them and the fire. Although previous bushfire disasters provide examples where people have survived by sheltering in a dam, swimming pool or river, these locations do not provide sufficient protection from smoke or radiant heat and should only be considered as an ‘absolute last resort’ (AFAC 2012). Meanwhile, sheltering in the bath or bathroom is a major cause of bushfire fatalities (Handmer et al. 2010; Haynes et al. 2010). On Black Saturday, for example, 46 of the 173 fatalities had been sheltering in baths or bathrooms at the time of death (Handmer et al. 2010; Teague et al. 2010). Consequently, sheltering in baths or bathrooms is not recommended under any circumstances (AFAC 2012).

For the children in the current study, underground bunkers usually took the form of cellars located beneath the house. Cellars were generally viewed as safe shelters because they tend to be composed of non-flammable materials, such as dirt or cement. However, as research has shown, even if a bunker doesn’t burn, occupants can be killed by the highly noxious gases that are emitted by other burning buildings (Australian Building Codes Board (ABCB) 2010; Handmer et al. 2010; Teague et al. 2010). Hence, Australian fire agencies strongly advise against sheltering in any bunker that has not been constructed according to strict building regulations (AFAC 2012). Even then, a bunker should only be viewed as a ‘place of last resort’ and never as a substitute for leaving early or staying to defend (ABCB 2010; AFAC 2012).

As noted earlier, some children viewed sheltering as a contingency plan to be implemented only when attempts at defending had failed. For others, however, sheltering constituted a first line of response. Children who viewed sheltering as a contingency plan had formed this view through their involvement in household emergency planning. However, the circumstances of children for whom it was the preferred mode of response were more ambiguous. Some children reported that their household plan, as developed by their parents, involved passive sheltering. However, the parents of these children were not interviewed, so this could not be verified. That said, previous research has found that in some households, sheltering is viewed as an appropriate first line of response (Handmer et al. 2010). Therefore, it is possible that these children were providing accurate accounts of their household plans.


Implications for research, policy and practice

The findings of this paper provide important insights into children’s knowledge of emergency bushfire response and the processes that influence knowledge development in this domain. Through their engagement in the research process, the children in this study articulated their perspectives on three key approaches to emergency response – leaving, staying to defend and sheltering. For each approach, however, there was substantial variation in the accuracy and sophistication of children’s knowledge. Much of this variation was explained by the extent to which they had been involved in household bushfire planning, which is consistent with a robust literature on the sociocultural nature of knowledge development (Cole 1996; Scribner and Tobach 1997; Rogoff 2003; Lightfoot et al. 2012). When children had been afforded a high level of involvement, they exhibited more sophisticated understandings that were largely consistent with the advice of Australian fire agencies. Importantly, age did not exert a direct influence on the sophistication of children’s knowledge: however, in some cases, it had influenced parent decisions about the extent to which they had involved their children, suggesting that age is a key moderating variable.

When children had not been involved in household bushfire planning, their knowledge of bushfire response often comprised serious misconceptions. These misconceptions were frequently underpinned by flawed assumptions about the physical characteristics of bushfire hazards, highlighting the influence of existing conceptual networks on the interpretation of new information and novel situations (Donaldson 1978; Von Glasersfeld 1991; Bruner 1996; Piercey and Berlyne 2001; Kushnir and Xu 2012). When children underestimated the speed at which fires can travel or the intensity and magnitude of smoke and radiant heat, they tended to advocate dangerous ‘wait and see’ approaches to leaving. Similarly, when they believed that direct exposure to the fire front was the main cause of house ignition, they tended to retain last-minute evacuation as a viable approach to staying to defend. Flawed assumptions about bushfire hazards also underpinned the common misconception that passive sheltering constitutes an appropriate first line of response. Importantly, numerous studies of bushfire disasters have attributed a large proportion of fatalities to a general lack of knowledge about the specifics of bushfire hazards (McLennan et al. 2009; Handmer et al. 2010). Therefore, the physical characteristics of bushfire hazards should be made a core component of school-based bushfire education.

The influence of children’s existing knowledge on the interpretation of new information and novel situations was further evidenced by the application of house-fire escape plans to the bushfire context. As demonstrated by children’s detailed accounts of well-developed house-fire escape plans, school-based house-fire education has been highly successful in conveying the key concepts of house-fire safety. Given that children are far more likely to be killed or injured in a house fire than a bushfire (Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) 2005; Haynes et al. 2010a), it is essential that bushfire education does not undermine this success. This will require education programs that highlight the fundamental differences between house-fire and bushfire emergencies and explicitly dispel any notion that house-fire escape plans can be appropriately applied in the bushfire context. Ensuring that children understand the physical characteristics of bushfire hazards will likely assist this process.

The results suggest that the success of house-fire education has been partly due to homework activities that encourage households to develop house-fire escape plans. Although it has been suggested that a similar approach could be used to encourage household planning and preparedness for bushfire (Teague et al. 2010; AFAC 2014), there are some important caveats. Corroborating previous research (e.g. Finnis et al. 2010; Ronan et al. 2010), the children in the current study suggested that understanding their household bushfire plan had assuaged their bushfire-related fears. However, not all parents had sought to involve their children and some had made a conscious decision not to do so. Thus, home-based bushfire planning activity will need to be accompanied by evidence-based information on benefits of involving children, as well as practical advice on how to do this effectively (cf. Bushfire Cooperative Research Centre (CRC) 2013). For children who might struggle to engage their parents, home-based activities will also need to include straightforward planning activities that children can carry out relatively independently, such as packing a firebox. Importantly, the psychosocial benefits of packing a firebox should not be underestimated: first, it would bolster children’s sense of self-efficacy, which is a key predictor of future preparedness behaviours (Citizen Corps/Federal Emergency Management Agency (FEMA) 2008; Solberg et al. 2010); and second, in the event of an actual disaster, the protection of valued possessions would act as a buffer against the development of trauma symptomology (Woolsey and Bracy 2010; Osofsky and Osofsky 2013).

In the present research, the application of qualitative methods enabled the identification of clear trends in children’s misconceptions. However, the identification of these trends should not be viewed as a substitute for bushfire education that facilitates genuine dialogue between children and educators. Bushfire education that advances children’s knowledge will require learning activities that encourage each child to voice their perspectives and allow the idiosyncratic nature of their misconceptions to be understood. This precludes taking the misconceptions documented in this paper and presenting them to children as ‘myths’ to be ‘debunked’. Such an approach would only circumvent the kind of dialogue that promotes knowledge development (Cole 1996; Rogoff 2003). As demonstrated by Rogoff et al. (1993), knowledge development requires a ‘stretch’ on the part of both children and educators: educators must stretch themselves ‘downward’ to understand how the child understands the task or problem, whereas the children must stretch themselves ‘upward’ in the direction of a more mature definition. Thus, opportunities for genuine dialogue should permeate every element of children’s bushfire education.

Although this research has identified key issues that should be considered in the design and delivery of children’s bushfire education, there is an urgent need for further research in this area. First, the aim of this research was theory development, which required the use of inductive research methods. However, the emergent theoretical insights should now be subjected to rigorous testing through the use of hypothetico-deductive methodologies and quantitative methods. Second, the results represent the knowledge and experiences of a discrete demographic cohort (i.e. predominantly white, English-speaking, 8- to 12-year-old children). Furthermore, although boys and girls were equally represented in the sample, the data were not analysed through a gendered lens. Yet personal characteristics such as age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, ethnicity and disability strongly influence how people experience hazards and disasters (Hewitt 1997; Wisner et al. 2004; Collins 2005; Eriksen et al. 2010; Whittaker et al. 2012). Therefore, this research should be extended to incorporate gendered analyses of both younger children and adolescents from a wider range of socioeconomic and cultural backgrounds.

There is also a need for policy research that more clearly defines children’s roles in bushfire response. Although several families in this study had involved their children in plans to stay and defend, the official consensus is that children should be kept well away from any bushfire threat (CFA 2013). At the same time, fire agencies advise residents to decide what they will do if they cannot leave (AFAC 2012; CFA 2013). Yet there is no advice for how families with children should manage this contingency. Although the analysis of Black Saturday fatalities highlighted the dangers of leaving children unattended inside homes (Handmer et al. 2010; Teague et al. 2010), the actions that contributed to children’s survival were not thoroughly examined. Further research in this area would enable evidence-based messaging and provide a more solid foundation for child-centred policy development.

Finally, a major limitation of this study is that neither teachers nor emergency managers were interviewed, thereby precluding an examination of the contextual factors that influence the development and delivery of school-based bushfire education. In the international literature, a general lack of hazards education in schools has been attributed to an overcrowded curriculum, an absence of quality teaching resources and a lack of knowledge and awareness among teachers (Shaw et al. 2011; Selby and Kagawa 2013). Yet none of these explanations have been substantiated by empirical research. If the most recent recommendation of the Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission is to be fully implemented, the contextual factors influencing the delivery of school-based bushfire education will need to identified and addressed.


Conclusion

Although Australian children have been marginalised and excluded from academic and political discussions of community bushfire safety, this research suggests that they should be afforded a more significant role. Although children’s knowledge was often characterised by gaps and misconceptions, they also demonstrated a capacity for understanding the fundamental principles of safe emergency response, particularly when they had been involved in household bushfire planning. As such, children represent a major resource for the development of safer, more resilient communities. Although the increased commitment to delivering bushfire education in Australian schools represents a valuable opportunity to capitalise on this resource, education programs will need to accommodate children’s existing knowledge as well as the social contexts of their everyday lives. This will require learning activities that facilitate genuine dialogue between children, parents and educators and respect the diverse perspectives and experiences of these various stakeholders.



Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Bushfire Co-operative Research Centre and the University of Tasmania. The author would like to thank the anonymous reviewers and the Editor and Associate Editor of the International Journal of Wildland Fire for their valuable feedback on the original manuscript.


References

ABCB (2010) Performance standard for private bushfire shelters. (Commonwealth and State and Territory Governments of Australia: Canberra, ACT) Available at http://www.abcb.gov.au/education-events-resources/publications/~/media/Files/Download%20Documents/Education%20and%20Training/Handbooks/2010_Performance_Standard_for_PBS.ashx [Verified 3 February 2015]

ABS (2005) Mortality and morbidity: children’s accidents and injuries. Available at http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/2f762f95845417aeca25706c00834efa/1d72f5e5299decc5ca25703b0080ccbf!OpenDocument [Verified 3 February 2015]

AFAC (2012) Bushfires and community safety: position 5.0. (AFAC: East Melbourne, Vic.) Available at http://www.afac.com.au/docs/position/bushfires-community-safety.pdf?sfvrsn=20 [Verified 3 February 2015]

AFAC (2014) Principles for educating children in natural hazards and other emergencies. Available at http://www.afac.com.au/docs/position/educating-children-in-emergencies.pdf?sfvrsn=8 [Verified 3 February 2015]

American Bar Association (2009) ‘Children, Law and Disasters: What Have We Learned from Hurricane Katrina?’ (American Bar Association/University of Houston: Houston, TX)

Anderson W (2005) Bringing children into focus on the social science disaster research agenda. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters 23, 159–175.

Blanchi R, Leonard J (2008) Property safety: judging structural safety. In ‘Community Bushfire Safety’. (Eds J Handmer, K Haynes) pp. 77–78. (CSIRO Publishing: Melbourne)

Blanchi R, Lucas C, Leonard J, Finkele K (2010) Meteorological conditions and wildfire-related house loss in Australia. International Journal of Wildland Fire 19, 914–926.
Meteorological conditions and wildfire-related house loss in Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bruner J (1996) ‘The Culture of Education.’ (Harvard University Press: Boston, MA)

Bureau of Meteorology (2014) Fire weather warnings. Available at http://www.bom.gov.au/weather-services/bushfire/index.shtml [Verified 3 February 2015]

Bushfire CRC (2013) Making a bushfire plan? Involve your kids. Available at http://www.bushfirecrc.com/resources/product/involve-your-kids-bushfire-survival-planning-ebook [Verified 3 February 2015]

CFA (2014) Fire Safe Kids. (CFA: Burwood, Vic.) Available at http://www.cfa.vic.gov.au/kids-schools/fire-safe-kids/ [Verified 3 February 2015]

CFA (2013) Fire Ready Kit. (CFA: Burwood, Vic.) Available at http://www.cfa.vic.gov.au/fm_files/attachments/plan_and_prepare/frk_2013/4713_CFA_2014_FireReadyKit_web.pdf [Verified 3 February 2015]

Charmaz K (2006) ‘Constructing Grounding Theory.’ (Sage: London)

Citizen Corps/(FEMA) (2008) ‘Citizen Preparedness Review. Community Resilience Through Civic Responsibility and Self-Reliance.’ (FEMA: Washington, DC)

Cole M (1996) ‘Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline.’ (Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA)

Collins T (2005) Households, forests, and fire hazard vulnerability in the American West: a case study of a California community. Environmental Hazards 6, 23–37.
Households, forests, and fire hazard vulnerability in the American West: a case study of a California community.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Corsaro W (2005) ‘The Sociology of Childhood.’ (Pine Forge Press: Thousand Oaks, CA)

Coyne IT (2006) Consultation with children in hospital: children, parents’ and nurses’ perspectives. Journal of Clinical Nursing 15, 61–71.
Consultation with children in hospital: children, parents’ and nurses’ perspectives.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Curtin C (2001) Eliciting children’s voices in qualitative research. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 55, 295–302.
Eliciting children’s voices in qualitative research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3MnnvVWrsQ%3D%3D&md5=c2d2456cc60870c9a7f9d6b261e1e3afCAS | 11723970PubMed |

Donaldson M (1978) ‘Children’s Minds.’ (Croom Helm: London)

Eder D, Fingerson L (2002) Interviewing children and adolescents. In ‘Handbook of Interview Research: Context and Method’. (Eds JF Gubrium, JA Holstein) pp. 181–201. (Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA)

Eriksen C, Gill N, Head L (2010) The gendered dimensions of bushfire in changing rural landscapes in Australia. Journal of Rural Studies 26, 332–342.
The gendered dimensions of bushfire in changing rural landscapes in Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Finnis K, Standring S, Johnston D, Ronan K (2004) Children’s understanding of natural hazards in Christchurch, New Zealand. Australian Journals of Emergency Management 19, 11–20.

Finnis K, Johnston D, Ronan K, White J (2010) Hazard perceptions and preparedness of Taranaki youth. Disaster Prevention and Management 19, 175–184.
Hazard perceptions and preparedness of Taranaki youth.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Glaser B, Strauss A (1967) ‘The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research.’ (Aldine: Chicago, IL)

Greig A, Taylor J, MacKay T (2007) ‘Doing Research with Children.’ (Sage Publications: London)

Gullone E (2000) The development of normal fear: a century of research. Clinical Psychology Review 20, 429–451.
The development of normal fear: a century of research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3cvls1Siug%3D%3D&md5=8adebe79327d687ec82f0575dbb06006CAS | 10832548PubMed |

Handmer J, Haynes K (2008) Interface (urban–rural fringe) community bushfire safety. In ‘Community Bushfire Safety’. (Eds J Handmer, K Haynes) pp. 3–10. (CSIRO Publishing: Melbourne)

Handmer J, Penning-Rowsell E (1990) ‘Hazards and the Communication of Risk.’ (Gower Technical: Brookfield, VT)

Handmer J, Tibbits A (2005) Is staying at home the safest option during bushfires? Historical evidence for an Australian approach. Environmental Hazards 6, 81–91.
Is staying at home the safest option during bushfires? Historical evidence for an Australian approach.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Handmer J, O’Neil S, Killalea D (2010) Review of fatalities in the February 7, 2009, bushfires. (Centre For Risk and Community Safety, RMIT University and Bushfire CRC: East Melbourne, Vic.) Available at http://www.royalcommission.vic.gov.au/Documents/Document-files/Exhibits/EXP-029-001-0001 [Verified 3 February 2015]

Haynes K, Barclay J, Pidgeon N (2008) Whose reality counts? Factors affecting the perception of volcanic risk. Journal of Volcanology and Geothermal Research 172, 259–272.
Whose reality counts? Factors affecting the perception of volcanic risk.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD1cXlvVOntL8%3D&md5=192d49666c027c4c2452da0524179383CAS |

Haynes K, Handmer J, McAneney J, Tibbits A, Coates L (2010a) Australian bushfire fatalities 1900–2008: exploring trends in relation to the ‘Prepare, stay and defend or leave early’ policy. Environmental Science & Policy 13, 185–194.
Australian bushfire fatalities 1900–2008: exploring trends in relation to the ‘Prepare, stay and defend or leave early’ policy.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Haynes K, Lassa J, Towers B (2010b) Child-centred disaster risk reduction and climate change adaptation: roles of gender and culture in Indonesia. Working Paper prepared for the Children in a Changing Climate Coalition, United Kingdom (Institute of Development Studies: Brighton, UK) Available at http://www.childreninachangingclimate.org/database/ids/Publications/Gender%20and%20Religion%20WP2%20WEB_PDF.pdf [Verified 3 February 2015]

Hennessey E, Heary C (2005) Exploring children’s views through focus groups. In ‘Researching Children’s Experience’. (Eds S Greene, D Hogan) pp. 236–252. (Sage: London)

Hewitt K (1997) ‘Regions of Risk.’ (Longman: Harlow, UK)

Horstman M, Aldiss S, Richardson A, Gibson F (2008) Methodological issues when using the Draw and Write technique with children aged 6 to 12 years. Qualitative Health Research 18, 1001–1011.
Methodological issues when using the Draw and Write technique with children aged 6 to 12 years.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 18552326PubMed |

James A, Prout A (2004) ‘Constructing and Reconstructing Childhood.’ (Pallgrave Macmillan: New York)

Kilmer R, Gil-Rivas V, Tedeschi R, Calhoun L (Eds) (2010) ‘Helping Families and Communities Recover From Disaster: Lessons Learned From Hurricane Katrina and its Aftermath.’ (American Psychological Association: Washington, DC)

Klein R (2009) Anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 50, 153–162.

Kushnir T, Xu F (2012) ‘Rational Constructivism in Cognitive Development.’ (Academic Press: Waltham, MA)

Leonard J (2003) People and property: a researcher’s perspective. In ‘Australia burning: fire ecology, policy and management issues’. (Eds G Cary, D Lindenmayer, S. Dovers) pp. 103–12. (CSIRO Publishing: Collingwood, Vic.)

Lightfoot C, Cole M, Cole S (2012) ‘The Development of Children.’ (Worth: Duffield, UK)

Masten A, Narayan A (2012) Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: pathways of risk and resilience. Annual Review of Psychology 63, 227–257.
Child development in the context of disaster, war, and terrorism: pathways of risk and resilience.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21943168PubMed |

McDermott BM, Lee EM, Judd M, Gibbon P (2005) Post-traumatic stress disorder and general psychopathology in children and adolescents following a wildfire disaster. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 50, 137–143.

McFarlane A (1987) Post-traumatic phenomena in a longitudinal study of children following a natural disaster. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 26, 764–769.
Post-traumatic phenomena in a longitudinal study of children following a natural disaster.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaL1c%2Fjt1Ortw%3D%3D&md5=aebe9f6ad4078e37048eb1e66d941f8cCAS | 3667509PubMed |

McLennan J, Omodei M, Elliott G, Holgate A (2009) ‘Deep survival’: experiences of some who lived when they might have died in the 7 February 2009 bushfires. Australian Journal of Emergency Management 26, 41–46.

Mitchell T, Haynes K, Hall N, Choong W, Oven K (2008) The role of children and youth in communicating disaster risk. Children, Youth and Environments 20, 1–30.

NHMRC (2014) National statement on ethical conduct in human research. (Australian Government: Canberra, ACT) Available at http://www.nhmrc.gov.au/guidelines/publications/e72 [Verified 3 February 2015]

Osofsky J, Osofsky H (2013) Lessons learned about the impact of disasters on children and families and post-disaster recovery. In ‘Child and Family Advocacy: Bridging the Gaps Between Research, Practice, and Policy’. (Ed. A Culp) pp. 91–105. (Springer: New York)

Peek L (2008) Children and disasters: understanding vulnerability, developing capacities, and promoting resilience – an introduction. Children, Youth and Environments 18, 1–29.

Peek L, Fothergill A (2009) Using focus groups: lessons from studying daycare centers, 9/11, and Hurricane Katrina. Qualitative Research 9, 31–59.
Using focus groups: lessons from studying daycare centers, 9/11, and Hurricane Katrina.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Perry R, Mushkatel A (1986) ‘Minority Citizens in Disasters.’ (University of Georgia: Athens, GA)

Pfefferbaum B, Weems C, Scott B, Nitiéma P, Noffsinger M, Pfefferbaum R, Varma V, Chakraburtty A (2013) Research methods in child disaster studies: a review of studies generated by the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks; the 2004 Indian Ocean tsunami; and Hurricane Katrina. Child and Youth Care Forum 42, 285–337.
Research methods in child disaster studies: a review of studies generated by the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks; the 2004 Indian Ocean tsunami; and Hurricane Katrina.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 24443635PubMed |

Piercey M, Berlyne DE (Transl) (2001) ‘The Psychology of Intelligence’ by J Piaget. (Routledge: London)

Pridmore P, Lansdown R (1997) Exploring children’s perceptions of health: does drawing really break down barriers? Health Education Journal 56, 219–230.
Exploring children’s perceptions of health: does drawing really break down barriers?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Qvortrup J (1994) Childhood matters: an introduction. In ‘Childhood Matters: Social Theory, Practice and Politics’. (Eds J Qvortrup, M Bardy, G Sgritta, H Wintersberger) pp. 1–24. (Avebury: Aldershot, UK)

Rogoff B (2003) ‘The Cultural Nature of Human Development.’ (Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK)

Rogoff B, Mistry J, Göncü A, Mosier C, Chavajay P, Brice Heath S (1993) Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58, i–179.
Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ronan K, Johnston D (2001) Correlates of hazard education programs for youth. Risk Analysis 21, 1055–1063.
Correlates of hazard education programs for youth.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD38%2FpsVymug%3D%3D&md5=69fca84520f753c034016f948c5706d0CAS | 11824681PubMed |

Ronan K, Johnston D (2003) Hazards education for youth: a quasi- experimental investigation. Risk Analysis 23, 1009–1020.
Hazards education for youth: a quasi- experimental investigation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 12969415PubMed |

Ronan K, Johnston D (2005) ‘Promoting Community Resilience in Disasters: The Role for Schools, Youth and Families.’ (Springer: New York)

Ronan K, Towers B (2014) Systems education for a sustainable planet: preparing children for natural disasters. Systems 2, 1–23.
Systems education for a sustainable planet: preparing children for natural disasters.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ronan K, Johnston D, Daly M, Fairley R (2001) School children’s risk perceptions and preparedness: a hazards education survey. Australasian Journal of Disaster and Trauma Studies 1, 1–32.

Ronan K, Crellin K, Johnston D (2010) Correlates of hazards education for youth: a replication study. Natural Hazards 53, 503–526.
Correlates of hazards education for youth: a replication study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sartain S, Clarke C, Heyman R (2008) Hearing the voices of children with chronic illness. Journal of Advanced Nursing 32, 913–921.

Scribner S, Tobach E (1997) ‘Mind and Social Practice: Selected Writings of Sylvia Scribner.’ (Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK)

Selby D, Kagawa F (2013) Disaster risk reduction in school curricula: case studies from thirty countries. (UNESCO: Paris). Available at http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0021/002170/217036e.pdf [Verified 3 February 2015]

Shaw R, Shiwaku K, Takeuchi Y (2011) ‘Community, Environment and Disaster Risk Management, Vol. 7 – Disaster Education.’ (Springer: London)

Shaw J, Espinel Z, Shultz J (2012) ‘Care of Children Exposed to the Traumatic Effects of Disaster.’ (American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC)

Solberg C, Rossetto T, Joffe H (2010) The social psychology of seismic hazard adjustment: re-evaluating the international literature. Natural Hazards and Earth System Sciences 10, 1663–1677.
The social psychology of seismic hazard adjustment: re-evaluating the international literature.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Strauss A, Corbin J (1998) ‘Basics of Qualitative Research: Grounded Theory Procedures and Techniques.’ 2nd edn (Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA)

Strauss A, Corbin J (2008) ‘Basics of Qualitative Research: Grounded Theory Procedures and Techniques.’ 3rd edn (Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA)

TasFire (2014) School programme: key home fire safety messages. Available at http://www.tfseducation.com.au/school-program/key-home-fire-safety-messages [Verified 3 February 2015]

Teague B, McLeod R, Pascoe S (2010) ‘2009 Victorian Bushfires Royal Commission Final Report.’ (Parliament of Victoria: Melbourne)

Tibbits A, Whittaker J (2007) Stay and defend or leave early: policy problems and experiences during the 2003 Victorian bushfires. Environmental Hazards 7, 283–290.
Stay and defend or leave early: policy problems and experiences during the 2003 Victorian bushfires.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Tibbits A, Handmer J, Haynes K, Lowe T, Whittaker J (2008) Prepare, stay and defend or leave early: evidence for the Australian approach. In ‘Community Bushfire Safety’. (Eds J Handmer, K Haynes) pp. 59–71. (CSIRO Publishing: Melbourne)

Towers B (2012) Children’s knowledge of vulnerability and resilience to bushfires. Ph.D. thesis, University of Tasmania, Hobart.

Twigg J (2004) ‘Disaster Risk Reduction: Mitigation and Preparedness in Development and Emergency Programming.’ (Humanitarian Practice Network: London)

Von Glasersfeld E (1991) ‘Radical Constructivism in Mathematics Education.’ (Kluwer: Netherlands)

Whittaker J, Handmer J (2010) Community bushfire safety: a review of post-Black Saturday research. Australian Journal of Emergency Management 25, 7–13.

Whittaker J, Handmer J, Mercer D (2012) Vulnerability to bushfires in rural Australia: a case study from East Gippsland, Victoria. Journal of Rural Studies 28, 161–173.
Vulnerability to bushfires in rural Australia: a case study from East Gippsland, Victoria.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Whittaker J, Haynes K, Handmer J, McLennan J (2013) Community safety during the 2009 Australian ‘Black Saturday’ bushfires: an analysis of household preparedness and response. International Journal of Wildland Fire 22, 841–849.
Community safety during the 2009 Australian ‘Black Saturday’ bushfires: an analysis of household preparedness and response.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Wisner B, Blaikie P, Cannon T, Davis I (2004) ‘At Risk: Natural Hazards, People’s Vulnerability and Disasters.’ (Routledge: London)

Woodgate R (2000a) A critical review of qualitative research of qualitative research related to children’s experiences with cancer. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 17, 207–228.
A critical review of qualitative research of qualitative research related to children’s experiences with cancer.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3M%2FmtFGktA%3D%3D&md5=39d5122d87c25b2ceada9ca915e35d07CAS | 11063515PubMed |

Woodgate R (2000b) An introduction to conducting qualitative research in children with cancer. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing 17, 192–206.
An introduction to conducting qualitative research in children with cancer.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3M%2FmtFGktw%3D%3D&md5=6ea0d22d1d6f465799228857326fc80aCAS | 11063514PubMed |

Woolsey C, Bracy K (2010) Emergency response and the psychological needs of school-age children. Traumatology 16, 1–6.
Emergency response and the psychological needs of school-age children.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Yelland C, Robinson P, Lock C, La Greca A, Kokegei B, Ridgway V, Lai B (2010) Bushfire impact on youth. Journal of Traumatic Stress 23, 274–277.




1 After each focus group, time was always taken to explicitly address children’s misconceptions. Children were also encouraged to seek more information from their parents and teachers about bushfire emergency response and school principals were encouraged to engage with their local fire agency and take advantage of any school-based education programs on offer.