Register      Login
Wildlife Research Wildlife Research Society
Ecology, management and conservation in natural and modified habitats
RESEARCH ARTICLE

How quoll-ified are northern and spotted-tailed quoll detection dogs?

La Toya J. Jamieson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9257-8744 A C E , Amanda L. Hancock B , Greg S. Baxter A and Peter J. Murray https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1143-1706 A D
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A The University of Queensland, Wildlife Science Unit, School of Agriculture and Food Sciences, Gatton Campus, Qld 4343, Australia.

B Wompoo Farm Pty Ltd, Carnarvon Canines – Wildlife Detection Dog Team, 19 Arthys Road, Cooran, Qld 4569, Australia.

C Present address: Zoos Victoria, Wildlife Detection Dog Program, Life Sciences, Healesville Sanctuary, Vic. 3777, Australia.

D Present address: The University of Southern Queensland, School of Sciences, Darling Heights, Qld 4350, Australia.

E Corresponding author. Email: la.jamieson@uqconnect.edu.au

Wildlife Research 48(4) 376-384 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR19243
Submitted: 15 December 2019  Accepted: 16 November 2020   Published: 15 February 2021

Abstract

Context: Wildlife detection dogs have been used globally in environmental monitoring. However, their effectiveness in the Australian context has been only minimally researched. Increased understanding of detection dog accuracy and efficacy is required for their inclusion in survey guidelines used by proponents of referred actions potentially impacting Australia’s threatened mammals. Evaluation of new methods is also important for advancing population monitoring, particularly for threatened species.

Aims: To determine the efficacy of wildlife detection dogs as a survey tool for low-density, cryptic species, using northern (Dasyurus hallucatus) and spotted-tailed (D. maculatus) quolls as subjects. We compared detection dogs, human search effort and camera trapping results, in simulated accuracy and efficacy trials, and field surveys.

Methods: Two wildlife detection dogs’ scores for sensitivity (ability to identify a target species scat) and specificity (ability to distinguish target from non-target species scats) were calculated during accuracy trials. The dogs were tested using 288 samples, of which 32 were targets, where northern and spotted-tailed quoll scat were the targets in separate trials. Survey efficacy was determined following completing 12 simulated surveys (6 per target species) involving a single, randomly placed scat sample in a 1–1.5 ha search area. During the northern quoll simulated surveys the dogs’ survey efficacy was compared with that of a human surveyor. The dogs also undertook field surveys for both northern and spotted-tailed quolls, in conjunction with camera trapping for comparison.

Key results: During accuracy trials the dogs had an average sensitivity and specificity respectively of 100% and 98.4% for northern quoll, and 100% and 98% for spotted-tailed quoll. Their average search time in efficacy trials for northern quoll was 11.07 min (significantly faster than the human surveyor), and 2.98 min for spotted-tailed quoll in the 1–1.5 ha search areas. During field surveys, northern quoll scats were detected at sites where camera trapping failed to determine their presence. No spotted-tailed quoll scat was detected by the dogs during field surveys.

Conclusions: Trained and experienced detection dogs can work very accurately and efficiently, which is vital to their field success. Detection dogs are therefore capable of detecting evidence of species presence where alternative methods may be unsuccessful.

Implications: Our study supports the future use of highly trained detection dogs for wildlife surveys and monitoring in Australia. Our results demonstrate that detection dogs can be highly accurate and are a beneficial stand-alone or complimentary method.

Keywords: accuracy, detection dogs, efficacy, field surveys, quolls.


References

Austin, C., Tuft, K., Ramp, D., Cremona, T., and Webb, J. K. (2017). Bait preference for remote camera trap studies of the endangered northern quoll (Dasyurus hallucatus). Australian Mammalogy 39, 72–77.
Bait preference for remote camera trap studies of the endangered northern quoll (Dasyurus hallucatus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Australian Government (2011). Survey guidelines for Australia’s threatened mammals – Guidelines for detecting mammals listed as threatened under the EPBC Act 1999. Department of Sustainability, Environment, Water, Population and Communities, Canberra, ACT, Australia.

Australian Government (2016). Environment Protection and Biodiversity Conservation Act 1999 referral guidelines for the Endangered northern quoll (Dasyurus hallactus) – EPBC Act Policy Statement 3.25. Department of Sustainability, Environment, Water, Population and Communities, Canberra, ACT, Australia.

Bird, R. C. (1997). An examination of the training and reliability of the narcotics detection dog. Kentucky Law Journal (Lexington, Ky.) 85, 405–433.

Bolen, E. G., and Robinson, W. L. (2003). ‘Wildlife ecology and management.’ (Prentice Hall: NJ, USA.)

Browne, C. M., Stafford, K. J., and Fordham, R. A. (2015). The detection and identification of tuatara and gecko scent by dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research 10, 496–503.
The detection and identification of tuatara and gecko scent by dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cristescu, R. H., Foley, E., Markula, A., Jackson, G., Jones, D., and Frère, C. (2015). Accuracy and efficiency of detection dogs: a powerful new tool for koala conservation and management. Scientific Reports 5, 8349–8355.
Accuracy and efficiency of detection dogs: a powerful new tool for koala conservation and management.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25666691PubMed |

DeMatteo, K. E., Rinas, M. A., Sede, M. M., Davenport, B., Argüelles, C. F., Lovett, K., and Parker, P. G. (2009). Detection dogs: an effective technique for bush dog surveys. The Journal of Wildlife Management 73, 1436–1440.
Detection dogs: an effective technique for bush dog surveys.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

DeMatteo, K. E., Blake, L. W., Young, J. K., and Davenport, B. (2018). How behaviour of nontarget species affects perceived accuracy of scat detection dog surveys. Scientific Reports 8, e13830.
How behaviour of nontarget species affects perceived accuracy of scat detection dog surveys.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 30218000PubMed |

Duggan, J. M., Heske, E. J., Schooley, R. L., Hurt, A., and Whitelaw, A. (2011). Comparing detection dog and livetrapping surveys for a cryptic rodent. The Journal of Wildlife Management 75, 1209–1217.
Comparing detection dog and livetrapping surveys for a cryptic rodent.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Frederick, R. I., and Bowden, S. C. (2009). The test validation summary. Assessment 16, 215–236.
The test validation summary.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 18838580PubMed |

Fryxell, J. M., Sinclair, A. R. E., and Caughley, G. (2014). Wildlife ecology, conservation, and management. (John Wiley & Sons: West Sussex, UK.)

Glen, A. S., and Veltman, C. J. (2018). Search strategies for conservation detection dogs. Wildlife Biology 2018, 1–9.
Search strategies for conservation detection dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Greatbatch, I., Gosling, R., and Allen, S. (2015). Quantifying Search dog effectiveness in a terrestrial search and rescue environment. Wilderness & Environmental Medicine 26, 327–334.
Quantifying Search dog effectiveness in a terrestrial search and rescue environment.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Harrison, R. L. (2006). A comparison of survey methods for detecting bobcats. Wildlife Society Bulletin 34, 548–552.
A comparison of survey methods for detecting bobcats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Helton, W. S. (2009). Overview of scent detection work – issues and opportunities. In ‘Canine Ergonomics: The Science of Working Dogs’. (Ed. W. S. Helton.) pp. 83–97. (CRC Press: London, UK.)

Hohnen, R., Ashby, J., Tuft, K., and McGregor, H. (2013). Individual identification of northern quolls (Dasyurus hallucatus) using remote cameras. Australian Mammalogy 35, 131–135.
Individual identification of northern quolls (Dasyurus hallucatus) using remote cameras.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hollerbach, L., Heurich, M., Reiners, T. B., and Nowak, C. (2018). Detection dogs allow for systematic non-invasive collection of DNA samples from Eurasian lynx. Mammalian Biology 90, 42–46.
Detection dogs allow for systematic non-invasive collection of DNA samples from Eurasian lynx.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Hurt, A., Woollett (Smith), D. A., and Parker, M. (2016). Training considerations in wildlife detection. In ‘Canine Olfaction Science and Law’. (Ed. J. Ensminger, T. Jezierski, and L. E. Papet.) pp. 139–153. (CRC Press: London, UK.)

Hurt, A., and Smith, D. A. (2009). Conservation dogs. In ‘Canine Ergonomics: The Science of Working Dogs’. (Ed. W. S. Helton.) pp. 175–194. (CRC Press: London, UK.)

Jamieson, L. J. (2019). ‘Improving wildlife detection dog team selection and training.’ PhD Thesis, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Qld, Australia10.14264/uql.2019.896

Jamieson, L. J., Baxter, G. S., and Murray, P. J. (2017). Identifying suitable detection dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 195, 1–7.
Identifying suitable detection dogs.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Jamieson, L. J., Baxter, G. S., and Murray, P. J. (2018a). You are not my handler! Impact of changing handlers on dogs’ behaviours and detection performance. Animals (Basel) 8, 176–187.
You are not my handler! Impact of changing handlers on dogs’ behaviours and detection performance.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Jamieson, L. J., Baxter, G. S., and Murray, P. J. (2018b). Who’s a good handler? Important skills and personality profiles of wildlife detection dog handlers. Animals (Basel) 8, 222–236.
Who’s a good handler? Important skills and personality profiles of wildlife detection dog handlers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Johnen, D., Heuwieser, W., and Fischer-Tenhagen, C. (2013). Canine scent detection – fact or fiction? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 148, 201–208.
Canine scent detection – fact or fiction?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Johnen, D., Heuwieser, W., and Fischer-Tenhagen, C. (2015). How to train a dog to detect cows in heat – training and success. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 171, 39–46.
How to train a dog to detect cows in heat – training and success.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Johnen, D., Heuwieser, W., and Fischer-Tenhagen, C. (2017). An approach to identify bias in scent detection dog training. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 189, 1–12.
An approach to identify bias in scent detection dog training.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kapfer, J. M., Munoz, D. J., and Tomasek, T. (2012). Use of wildlife detector dogs to study Eastern box turtle (Terrapene carolina carolina) populations. Herpetological Conservation and Biology 7, 169–175.

Kerley, L. L. (2010). Using dogs for tiger conservation and research. Integrative Zoology 5, 390–396.
Using dogs for tiger conservation and research.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21392356PubMed |

Lit, L., Schweitzer, J. B., and Oberbauer, A. M. (2011). Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes. Animal Cognition 14, 387–394.
Handler beliefs affect scent detection dog outcomes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21225441PubMed |

Long, R. A., Donovan, T. M., Mackay, P., Buzas, J. S., and Zielinski, W. J. (2007a). Comparing detection dogs, cameras, and hair snares for surveying carnivores. The Journal of Wildlife Management 71, 2018–2025.
Comparing detection dogs, cameras, and hair snares for surveying carnivores.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Long, R. A., Donovan, T. M., Mackay, P., Zielinski, W. J., and Buzas, J. S. (2007b). Effectiveness of scat detection dogs for detecting forest carnivores. The Journal of Wildlife Management 71, 2007–2017.
Effectiveness of scat detection dogs for detecting forest carnivores.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Meek, P. D., Ballard, G., Fleming, P. J. S., Schaefer, M., Williams, W., and Falzon, G. (2014). Camera traps can be heard and seen by animals. PLoS One 9, e110832.
Camera traps can be heard and seen by animals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 25354356PubMed |

Meek, P. D., Ballard, G., and Fleming, P. J. S. (2015). The pitfalls of wildlife camera trapping as a survey tool in Australia. Australian Mammalogy 37, 13–22.
The pitfalls of wildlife camera trapping as a survey tool in Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Oakwood, M. (2002). Spatial and social organization of a carnivorous marsupial Dasyurus hallucatus (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae). Journal of Zoology 257, 237–248.
Spatial and social organization of a carnivorous marsupial Dasyurus hallucatus (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Oakwood, M. (2008). Northern quoll, Dasyurus hallucatus. In ‘Mammals of Australia’. (Ed. S. Van Dyck, and R. Strahan.) pp. 57–59. (Reed Books: Sydney, NSW, Australia.)

Oakwood, M., Bradley, A. J., and Cockburn, A. (2001). Semelparity in a large marsupial. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 268, 407–411.
Semelparity in a large marsupial.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Oesterhelweg, L., Kröber, S., Rottmann, K., Willhöft, J., Braun, C., Thies, N., Püschel, K., Silkenath, J., and Gehl, A. (2008). Cadaver dogs – a study on detection of contaminated carpet squares. Forensic Science International 174, 35–39.
Cadaver dogs – a study on detection of contaminated carpet squares.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 17403590PubMed |

Oliveira, M. L. D., Norris, D., Ramírez, J. F. M., Peres, P. H. D. F., Galetti, M., and Duarte, J. M. B. (2012). Dogs can detect scat samples more efficiently than humans: an experiment in a continuous Atlantic Forest remnant. Zoologia (Curitiba) 29, 183–186.
Dogs can detect scat samples more efficiently than humans: an experiment in a continuous Atlantic Forest remnant.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Orkin, J. D., Yang, Y., Yang, C., Yu, D. W., and Jiang, X. (2016). Cost-effective scat-detection dogs: unleashing a powerful new tool for international mammalian conservation biology. Scientific Reports 6, 34758.
Cost-effective scat-detection dogs: unleashing a powerful new tool for international mammalian conservation biology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 27721442PubMed |

Papet, L. E., and Minhinnick, S. (2016). Training a statistically superior scent discrimination canine – where trainer wisdom meets scientific validation. In ‘Canine Olfaction Science and Law’. (Eds J. Ensminger, T. Jezierski, and L. E. Papet.) pp. 173–195. (CRC Press: London, UK.)

Rolland, R. M., Hamilton, P. K., Kraus, S. D., Davenport, B., Gillett, R. M., and Wasser, S. M. (2006). Faecal sampling using detection dogs to study reproduction and health in North Atlantic right whales (Eubalaena glacialis). The Journal of Cetacean Research and Management 8, 121–125.

Séquin, E. S., Jaeger, M. M., Brussard, P. F., and Barrett, R. H. (2003). Wariness of coyotes to camera traps relative to social status and territory boundaries. Canadian Journal of Zoology 81, 2015–2025.
Wariness of coyotes to camera traps relative to social status and territory boundaries.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Syrotuck, W. (1972). Scent and the Scenting Dog. (Barkleigh Productions Inc.: Pennsylvania).

Tobler, M. Q., Carrillo-Percastegui, S. E., Leite Pitman, R., Mares, R., and Powell, G. (2008). An evaluation of camera traps for inventorying large- and medium-sized terrestrial rainforest mammals. Animal Conservation 11, 169–178.
An evaluation of camera traps for inventorying large- and medium-sized terrestrial rainforest mammals.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Turpin, J. M., and Bamford, M. J. (2015). A new population of the northern quoll (Dasyurus hallucatus) on the edge of the Little Sandy Desert, Western Australia. Australian Mammalogy 37, 86–91.
A new population of the northern quoll (Dasyurus hallucatus) on the edge of the Little Sandy Desert, Western Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Vynne, C., Skalsji, J. R., Machado, R. B., Groom, M. J., Jacomo, A. T. A., Marinho, J., Neto, M. B. R., Pomilla, C., Silveira, L., Smith, H., and Wasser, S. K. (2011). Effectiveness of scat-detection dogs in determining species presence in a tropical savanna landscape. Conservation Biology 25, 154–162.
Effectiveness of scat-detection dogs in determining species presence in a tropical savanna landscape.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 21029162PubMed |

Wasser, S. K., Davenport, B., Ramage, E. R., Hunt, K. E., Parker, M., Clarke, C., and Stenhouse, G. (2004). Scat detection dogs in wildlife research and management: application to grizzly and black bears in the Yellowhead Ecosystem, Alberta, Canada. Canadian Journal of Zoology 82, 475–492.
Scat detection dogs in wildlife research and management: application to grizzly and black bears in the Yellowhead Ecosystem, Alberta, Canada.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Williams, M., and Johnston, J. M. (2002). Training and maintaining the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) on an increasing number of odor discriminations in a controlled setting. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 78, 55–65.
Training and maintaining the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) on an increasing number of odor discriminations in a controlled setting.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |