Concordance between monetary and sexual delay discounting in men who have sex with men
Jeb Jones A D , Jodie L. Guest A C , Patrick S. Sullivan A , Michael R. Kramer A , Samuel M. Jenness A and Jessica M. Sales BA Department of Epidemiology, Emory University, 1518 Clifton Road, Atlanta, GA 30322.
B Department of Behavioral Sciences and Health Education, Emory University, 1518 Clifton Road, Atlanta, GA 30322.
C Department of Family and Preventative Medicine, Emory University, 1648 Pierce Dr NE, Atlanta, GA 30322.
D Corresponding author. Email: jeb.jones@emory.edu
Sexual Health 15(3) 214-222 https://doi.org/10.1071/SH17111
Submitted: 27 June 2017 Accepted: 15 September 2017 Published: 7 December 2017
Abstract
Background: Delay discounting has been found to be associated with numerous health-related outcomes, including risky sexual behaviour. To date, it is unclear whether delay discounting measured in different domains is associated within individuals. The goal of this study was to assess the concordance of monetary and sexual delay discounting in men who have sex with men. Methods: Participants completed an online survey, including the Monetary Choice Questionnaire and the Sexual Discounting Task. Linear regression models were used to assess the association between monetary and sexual discount rates. Results: Sexual discount rates did not predict monetary discount rates. There was a substantial amount of clustering of sexual discount rates, requiring sexual discounting data to be categorised. Conclusions: Monetary and sexual delay discounting are distinct processes that are not necessarily associated within individuals, and monetary delay discounting is not an appropriate proxy measure for sexual impulsivity. Data from the Sexual Discounting Task are typically rank-transformed for analysis. These data suggest that this might be an invalid method of analysis. Future studies should investigate the distribution of their data to determine if it is appropriate to analyse sexual discounting data as a continuous measure.
Additional keywords: sexual risk, HIV.
References
[1] Rosenberg ES, Grey JA, Sanchez TH, Sullivan PS. Rates of prevalent HIV infection, prevalent diagnoses and new diagnoses among men who have sex with men in US states, metropolitan statistical areas, and counties, 2012–2013. JMIR Public Health and Surveillance 2016; 2 e22| Rates of prevalent HIV infection, prevalent diagnoses and new diagnoses among men who have sex with men in US states, metropolitan statistical areas, and counties, 2012–2013.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[2] Johnson AS, Hall HI, Hu X, Lansky A, Holtgrave DR, Mermin J. Trends in diagnoses of HIV infection in the United States, 2002–2011. JAMA 2014; 312 432–4.
| Trends in diagnoses of HIV infection in the United States, 2002–2011.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC2MXns1Whur8%3D&md5=aeeb353f0cd82b333c8dc6370d190e9fCAS |
[3] Purcell DW, Johnson CH, Lansky A, Prejean J, Stein R, Denning P, Gau Z, Weinstock H, Su J, Crepaz N. Estimating the population size of men who have sex with men in the United States to obtain HIV and syphilis rates. Open AIDS J 2012; 6 98–107.
| Estimating the population size of men who have sex with men in the United States to obtain HIV and syphilis rates.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[4] Sullivan PS, Carballo-Diéguez A, Coates T, Goodreau SM, McGowan I, Sanders EJ, Smith A, Goswami P, Sanchez J. Successes and challenges of HIV prevention in men who have sex with men. Lancet 2012; 380 388–99.
| Successes and challenges of HIV prevention in men who have sex with men.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[5] Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. J Res Pers 2011; 45 259–68.
| A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[6] Madden GJ, Johnson PS. A delay-discounting primer. In Bickel GJMWK, editor. Impulsivity: the behavioral and neurological science of discounting. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 11–37.
[7] Tsukayama E, Duckworth AL, Kim B. Resisting everything except temptation: evidence and an explanation for domain‐specific impulsivity. Eur J Pers 2012; 26 318–34.
| Resisting everything except temptation: evidence and an explanation for domain‐specific impulsivity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[8] Odum AL, Madden GJ, Badger GJ, Bickel WK. Needle sharing in opioid-dependent outpatients: psychological processes underlying risk. Drug Alcohol Depend 2000; 60 259–66.
| Needle sharing in opioid-dependent outpatients: psychological processes underlying risk.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3M%2Fls1Skug%3D%3D&md5=f64731cb26eee8e0fd7d082f0bcd1e2aCAS |
[9] Odum AL, Madden GJ, Bickel WK. Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes. Nicotine Tob Res 2002; 4 295–303.
| Discounting of delayed health gains and losses by current, never- and ex-smokers of cigarettes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[10] Bickel WK, Marsch LA. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction 2001; 96 73–86.
| Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3M7jtlOqsg%3D%3D&md5=53fe6b5213738a6045695e248ff49a63CAS |
[11] Epstein LH, Richards JB, Saad FG, Paluch RA, Roemmich JN, Lerman C. Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2003; 11 131–8.
| Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[12] Heil SH, Johnson MW, Higgins ST, Bickel WK. Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addict Behav 2006; 31 1290–4.
| Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[13] Kirby KN, Petry NM. Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction 2004; 99 461–71.
| Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[14] Jarmolowicz DP, Cherry JB, Reed DD, Bruce JM, Crespi JM, Lusk JL, Bruce AS. Robust relation between temporal discounting rates and body mass. Appetite 2014; 78 63–7.
| Robust relation between temporal discounting rates and body mass.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[15] Lawyer SR, Boomhower SR, Rasmussen EB. Differential associations between obesity and behavioral measures of impulsivity. Appetite 2015; 95 375–82.
| Differential associations between obesity and behavioral measures of impulsivity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[16] Chesson HW, Leichliter JS, Zimet GD, Rosenthal SL, Bernstein DI, Fife KH. Discount rates and risky sexual behaviors among teenagers and young adults. J Risk Uncertain 2006; 32 217–30.
| Discount rates and risky sexual behaviors among teenagers and young adults.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[17] Lawyer SR. Probability and delay discounting of erotic stimuli. Behav Processes 2008; 79 36–42.
| Probability and delay discounting of erotic stimuli.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[18] Lawyer SR, Schoepflin FJ. Predicting domain-specific outcomes using delay and probability discounting for sexual versus monetary outcomes. Behav Processes 2013; 96 71–8.
| Predicting domain-specific outcomes using delay and probability discounting for sexual versus monetary outcomes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[19] Johnson MW, Bruner NR. The sexual discounting task: HIV risk behavior and the discounting of delayed sexual rewards in cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend 2012; 123 15–21.
| The sexual discounting task: HIV risk behavior and the discounting of delayed sexual rewards in cocaine dependence.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[20] Madden GJ, Bickel WK, Jacobs EA. Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients: exponential or hyperbolic discounting functions? Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 1999; 7 284–93.
| Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients: exponential or hyperbolic discounting functions?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK1MvgtFOgtQ%3D%3D&md5=b5cb7a17eee1fc9bbcfc5b4ad69931e1CAS |
[21] Madden GJ, Petry NM, Badger GJ, Bickel WK. Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 1997; 5 256–62.
| Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DyaK2sXlsVyiu7c%3D&md5=feebedcb6584213886f33fb200114778CAS |
[22] Petry NM. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 154 243–50.
| Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3MXjsVens7o%3D&md5=5ec61bae2363a1cdfa7b2b026ae65d30CAS |
[23] Petry NM. Discounting of money, health, and freedom in substance abusers and controls. Drug Alcohol Depend 2003; 71 133–41.
| Discounting of money, health, and freedom in substance abusers and controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[24] Black AC, Rosen MI. A money management-based substance use treatment increases valuation of future rewards. Addict Behav 2011; 36 125–8.
| A money management-based substance use treatment increases valuation of future rewards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[25] Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C. Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 69 260–5.
| Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[26] Lawyer SR, Williams SA, Prihodova T, Rollins JD, Lester AC. Probability and delay discounting of hypothetical sexual outcomes. Behav Processes 2010; 84 687–92.
| Probability and delay discounting of hypothetical sexual outcomes.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[27] Jarmolowicz DP, Bickel WK, Gatchalian KM. Alcohol-dependent individuals discount sex at higher rates than controls. Drug Alcohol Depend 2013; 131 320–3.
| Alcohol-dependent individuals discount sex at higher rates than controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[28] Jarmolowicz DP, Lemley SM, Asmussen L, Reed DD. Mr. right versus Mr. right now: a discounting-based approach to promiscuity. Behav Processes 2015; 115 117–22.
| Mr. right versus Mr. right now: a discounting-based approach to promiscuity.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[29] Dariotis JK, Johnson MW. Sexual discounting among high-risk youth ages 18–24: implications for sexual and substance use risk behaviors. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2015; 23 49–58.
| Sexual discounting among high-risk youth ages 18–24: implications for sexual and substance use risk behaviors.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[30] Herrmann ES, Johnson PS, Johnson MW. Examining delay discounting of condom-protected sex among men who have sex with men using crowdsourcing technology. AIDS Behav 2015; 19 1655–65.
| Examining delay discounting of condom-protected sex among men who have sex with men using crowdsourcing technology.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[31] Herrmann ES, Hand DJ, Johnson MW, Badger GJ, Heil SH. Examining delay discounting of condom-protected sex among opioid-dependent women and non-drug-using control women. Drug Alcohol Depend 2014; 144 53–60.
| Examining delay discounting of condom-protected sex among opioid-dependent women and non-drug-using control women.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[32] Johnson MW, Johnson PS, Herrmann ES, Sweeney MM. Delay and probability discounting of sexual and monetary outcomes in individuals with cocaine use disorders and matched controls. PLoS One 2015; 10 e0128641
| Delay and probability discounting of sexual and monetary outcomes in individuals with cocaine use disorders and matched controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[33] Koffarnus MN, Johnson MW, Thompson-Lake DG, Wesley MJ, Lohrenz T, Montague PR, Bickel WK. Cocaine-dependent adults and recreational cocaine users are more likely than controls to choose immediate unsafe sex over delayed safer sex. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2016; 24 297–304.
| Cocaine-dependent adults and recreational cocaine users are more likely than controls to choose immediate unsafe sex over delayed safer sex.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[34] Johnson PS, Sweeney MM, Herrmann ES, Johnson MW. Alcohol increases delay and probability discounting of condom-protected sex: a novel vector for alcohol-related HIV transmission. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2016; 40 1339–50.
| Alcohol increases delay and probability discounting of condom-protected sex: a novel vector for alcohol-related HIV transmission.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC28XovVylt78%3D&md5=b8dfc82a2fb442b476d3630b4f93798bCAS |
[35] Johnson MW, Herrmann ES, Sweeney MM, LeComte RS, Johnson PS. Cocaine administration dose-dependently increases sexual desire and decreases condom use likelihood: the role of delay and probability discounting in connecting cocaine with HIV. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2017; 234 599–612.
| Cocaine administration dose-dependently increases sexual desire and decreases condom use likelihood: the role of delay and probability discounting in connecting cocaine with HIV.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BC28XitVSrur3L&md5=ed885a485342f3f9abae5c38da318638CAS |
[36] Odum AL. Delay discounting: I’m a k, you’re a k. J Exp Anal Behav 2011; 96 427–39.
| Delay discounting: I’m a k, you’re a k.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[37] Jones J, Sullivan PS. Impulsivity as a risk factor for HIV transmission in men who have sex with men: a delay discounting approach. J Homosex 2015; 62 588–603.
| Impulsivity as a risk factor for HIV transmission in men who have sex with men: a delay discounting approach.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[38] Holt DD, Newquist MH, Smits RR, Tiry AM. Discounting of food, sex, and money. Psychon Bull Rev 2014; 21 794–802.
| Discounting of food, sex, and money.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[39] Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Sexually transmitted disease surveillance. Atlanta: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2016.
[40] Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen 1999; 128 78–87.
| Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DyaK1M3gtVWltw%3D%3D&md5=69d75d0526e16bdc057545a1eaba4c8eCAS |
[41] Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In Commons ML, Mazur JE, Nevin JA, Rachlin H, editors. Quantitative analyses of behavior: the effects of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1987. pp. 55–73.
[42] Kirby KN, Maraković NM. Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev 1996; 3 100–4.
| Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: rates decrease as amounts increase.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BC2c7lsVWmsQ%3D%3D&md5=b9ed0195f30ab393c704d01d523b1706CAS |
[43] Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. J Exp Anal Behav 2001; 76 235–43.
| Area under the curve as a measure of discounting.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:STN:280:DC%2BD3MrltlWmsA%3D%3D&md5=75d36506e6e8d9b6f4d20cd55bd7fdc3CAS |
[44] Conover WJ, Iman RL. Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and nonparametric statistics. Am Stat 1981; 35 124–9.
| Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and nonparametric statistics.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[45] Johnson MW, Bickel WK. An algorithm for identifying nonsystematic delay-discounting data. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2008; 16 264–74.
| An algorithm for identifying nonsystematic delay-discounting data.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[46] Baker F, Johnson MW, Bickel WK. Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. J Abnorm Psychol 2003; 112 382–92.
| Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[47] Dallery J, Raiff BR. Delay discounting predicts cigarette smoking in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007; 190 485–96.
| Delay discounting predicts cigarette smoking in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2sXhtFygsL0%3D&md5=cf942915e3b1dff17f819ca18a41ffa1CAS |
[48] Heyman GM, Gibb SP. Delay discounting in college cigarette chippers. Behav Pharmacol 2006; 17 669–79.
| Delay discounting in college cigarette chippers.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[49] Dassen FC, Houben K, Jansen A. Time orientation and eating behavior. Unhealthy eaters consider immediate consequences, while healthy eaters focus on future health. Appetite 2015; 91 13–19.
| Time orientation and eating behavior. Unhealthy eaters consider immediate consequences, while healthy eaters focus on future health.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[50] Dassen FC, Jansen A, Nederkoorn C, Houben K. Focus on the future: episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite 2016; 96 327–32.
[51] Hernandez-Romieu AC, Sullivan PS, Sanchez TH, Kelley CF, Peterson JL, Del Rio C, Salazar LF, Frew PM, Rosenberg ES. The comparability of men who have sex with men recruited from venue-time-space sampling and Facebook: a cohort study. JMIR Res Protoc 2014; 3 e37
| The comparability of men who have sex with men recruited from venue-time-space sampling and Facebook: a cohort study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |
[52] Booth-Kewley S, Edwards JE, Rosenfeld P. Impression management, social desirability, and computer administration of attitude questionnaires: does the computer make a difference? J Appl Psychol 1992; 77 562–6.
| Impression management, social desirability, and computer administration of attitude questionnaires: does the computer make a difference?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |