Register      Login
Wildlife Research Wildlife Research Society
Ecology, management and conservation in natural and modified habitats
RESEARCH ARTICLE

The use of scats as social signals in a solitary, endangered scincid lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis

Aaron L. Fenner A B and C. Michael Bull A
+ Author Affiliations
- Author Affiliations

A School of Biological Sciences, Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide, SA 5001, Australia.

B Corresponding author. Email: aaron.fenner@flinders.edu.au

Wildlife Research 37(7) 582-587 https://doi.org/10.1071/WR10122
Submitted: 23 July 2010  Accepted: 28 October 2010   Published: 17 December 2010

Abstract

Context: The deliberate accumulation of faeces or scats in one location is a common behaviour used for social signalling. The endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis, is a solitary-living species that appears to use scats as social signals. Previous studies have found that these lizards approach burrows more cautiously if there is a conspecific scat present and behave differently to scats from male and female conspecifics, indicating that these lizards can recognise scat-based conspecific cues. For endangered species, understanding how an animal uses and responds to chemical cues has the potential to become a powerful tool in conservation management.

Aims: To investigate whether these solitary lizards might be using scats as social signals by testing whether scats were being deposited in a non-random pattern designed to maximise their exposure to the nearest neighbour conspecifics.

Method: In the present study, we investigated the spatial pattern of scatting behaviour by pygmy bluetongue lizards in the field.

Results: Scats were consistently deposited in one direction that was significantly aligned with the nearest occupied burrow of a neighbouring lizard. The same deposition sites were used when previous scats were either destroyed by rain, or experimentally moved to a different location. Male lizards deposited scats significantly closer to their own burrows when neighbours were closer.

Key conclusions: The results were consistent with the lizards using scats to advertise their presence and status. The scat-deposition patterns observed in the present study suggest that scats are used as an olfactory signal associated with social organisation of pygmy bluetongue lizards.

Implications: Current plans for the conservation management of this endangered species include translocations. By understanding how these lizards respond to, and use chemical cues, we may be able to use this knowledge to aid in translocation programs, e.g. by pre-establishing territories for individuals before they are introduced into the population.

Additional keywords: dear enemy, pygmy bluetongue lizard, recognition, scat piles, social organisation.


References

Apio, A., Plath, M., and Wronski, T. (2006). Localised defecation sites: a tactic to avoid re-infection by gastro-intestinal tract parasites in bushbuck, Tragelaphus scriptus? Journal of Ethology 24, 85–90.
Localised defecation sites: a tactic to avoid re-infection by gastro-intestinal tract parasites in bushbuck, Tragelaphus scriptus?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Aragón, P., López, P., and Martin, J. (2000). Size dependent chemosensory responses to familiar and unfamiliar conspecific faecal pellets by the Iberian rock-lizard, Lacerta monticola. Ethology 106, 1115–1128.
Size dependent chemosensory responses to familiar and unfamiliar conspecific faecal pellets by the Iberian rock-lizard, Lacerta monticola.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Aragón, P., López, P., and Martin, J. (2001). Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50, 128–133.
Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Aragón, P., López, P., and Martin, J. (2003). Differential avoidance responses to chemical cues from familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by male Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola). Journal of Herpetology 37, 583–585.
Differential avoidance responses to chemical cues from familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by male Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Barwick, R. E. (1965). Studies on the scincid lizard, Egernia cunninghami (Gray, 1832). Australian National University, Canberra, ACT.

Bull, C. M., Griffin, C. L., and Johnston, G. R. (1999). Olfactory discrimination in scat-piling lizards. Behavioral Ecology 10, 136–140.
Olfactory discrimination in scat-piling lizards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bull, C. M., Griffin, C. L., Lanham, E. J., and Johnston, G. R. (2000). Recognition of pheromones from group members in a gregarious lizard, Egernia stokesii. Journal of Herpetology 34, 92–99.
Recognition of pheromones from group members in a gregarious lizard, Egernia stokesii.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Bull, C. M., Griffin, C. L., Bonnett, M., Gardner, M. G., and Cooper, S. J. B. (2001). Discrimination between related and unrelated individuals in the Australian lizard Egernia striolata. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50, 173–179.
Discrimination between related and unrelated individuals in the Australian lizard Egernia striolata.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Campbell-Palmer, R., and Rosell, F. (2010). Conservation of the Eurasian beaver Castor fiber: an olfactory perspective. Mammal Review 40, 293–312.
Conservation of the Eurasian beaver Castor fiber: an olfactory perspective.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Carpenter, G. C., and Duvall, D. (1995). Faecal scent marking in the western banded gecko (Coleonyx variegatus). Herpetologica 51, 33–38.

Chapple, D. G. (2003). Ecology, life-history, and behavior in the Australian scincid genus Egernia, with comments on the evolution of complex sociality in lizards. Herpetological Monograph 17, 145–180.
Ecology, life-history, and behavior in the Australian scincid genus Egernia, with comments on the evolution of complex sociality in lizards.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Cooper, W. E. (1996). Chemosensory recognition of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by the scincid lizard Eumeces laticeps. Ethology 102, 454–464.
Chemosensory recognition of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by the scincid lizard Eumeces laticeps.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Duffield, G. A., and Bull, C. M. (2002). Stable social aggregations in an Australian lizard, Egernia stokesii. Naturwissenschaften 89, 424–427.
Stable social aggregations in an Australian lizard, Egernia stokesii.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD38Xnt1ymsbw%3D&md5=c22b24be4c0a085ec1b890fec8c488ceCAS | 12435097PubMed |

Duvall, D., Graves, B. M., and Carpenter, G. C. (1987). Visual and chemical composite signaling effects of Sceloporus lizards fecal boli. Copeia 1987, 1028–1031.
Visual and chemical composite signaling effects of Sceloporus lizards fecal boli.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fellows, H. L., Fenner, A. L., and Bull, C. M. (2009). Spiders provide important resources for an endangered lizard. Journal of Zoology 279, 156–163.
Spiders provide important resources for an endangered lizard.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fenner, A. L. (2009). The biology, ecology and social structuring of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis: considerations for long term conservation. Ph.D. Thesis. Flinders University, Adelaide.

Fenner, A. L., and Bull, C. M. (2007). The impact of grassfire on fragmented populations of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis. Journal of Zoology 272, 444–450.
The impact of grassfire on fragmented populations of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fenner, A. L., and Bull, C. M. (2009). Tiliqua adelaidensis (pygmy bluetongue lizard). Mating behaviour. Herpetological Review 40, 91–92.

Fenner, A. L., and Bull, C. M. (2010a). Central-place territorial defence in a burrow dwelling lizard: aggressive responses to conspecific models in Tiliqua adelaidensis. Journal of Zoology , .
Central-place territorial defence in a burrow dwelling lizard: aggressive responses to conspecific models in Tiliqua adelaidensis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fenner, A. L., and Bull, C. M. (2010b). Responses of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard to conspecific scats. Journal of Ethology , .
Responses of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard to conspecific scats.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fenner, A. L., Bull, C. M., and Hutchinson, M. N. (2007). Omnivorous diet of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis. Amphibia-Reptilia 28, 560–565.
Omnivorous diet of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fenner, A. L., Smales, L. R., and Bull, C. M. (2008). Pharyngodon wandillahensis n. sp. (Nematoda: Pharyngodonidae) from the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard Tiliqua adelaidensis Peters, 1863 (Sauria: Scincidae), South Australia, Australia. Comparative Parasitology 75, 69–75.
Pharyngodon wandillahensis n. sp. (Nematoda: Pharyngodonidae) from the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard Tiliqua adelaidensis Peters, 1863 (Sauria: Scincidae), South Australia, Australia.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Ferkin, M. H. (1988). The effect of familiarity on social interactions in meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus: a laboratory and field study. Animal Behaviour 36, 1816–1822.
The effect of familiarity on social interactions in meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus: a laboratory and field study.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Fisher, J. (1954). Evolution and bird sociality. In ‘Evolution as a Process’. (Eds J. Huxley, A. C. Hardy and E. B. Ford.) pp. 71–83. (Allen and Unwin: London.)

Gardner, M. G., Bull, C. M., Fenner, A., and Donnellan, S. C. (2007). Consistent social structuring within aggregations of the Australian lizard Egernia stokesii across seven disconnected rocky outcrops. Journal of Ethology 25, 263–270.
Consistent social structuring within aggregations of the Australian lizard Egernia stokesii across seven disconnected rocky outcrops.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Gonzalo, A., Cabido, C., Martin, J., and López, P. (2004). Detection and discrimination of conspecific scents by the anguid slow-worm Anguis fragilis. Journal of Chemical Ecology 30, 1565–1573.
Detection and discrimination of conspecific scents by the anguid slow-worm Anguis fragilis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD2cXns1Oltbw%3D&md5=8b110128aa24e397f64846da621342c1CAS | 15537159PubMed |

Greer, A. E. (1989). ‘The Biology and Evolution of Australian Lizards.’ (Surrey Beatty: Sydney.)

Guillaume, O. (2000). Role of chemical communication and behavioural interactions among conspecifics in the choice of shelters by the cave-dwelling salmander Proteus anguinus (Caudata, Proteidae). Canadian Journal of Zoology 78, 167–173.
Role of chemical communication and behavioural interactions among conspecifics in the choice of shelters by the cave-dwelling salmander Proteus anguinus (Caudata, Proteidae).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD3cXivFajsbc%3D&md5=da40af4248b3d16a440dd2a91a137f6fCAS |

Guillaume, O. (2002). The importance of chemical communication in the social behaviour of cave salmanders. Comparisons between a strict (Proteus anguinus L., Proteidae) and a facultative (Euproctus asper D., Salamandridae) cave dweller. Bulletin De La Societe Zoologique De France 127, 263–272.

Hutchinson, M. N., Milne, T., and Croft, T. (1994). Redescription and ecological notes on the pygmy bluetongue Tiliqua adelaidensis (Squamata: Scincidae). Transactions of the Royal Society of South Australia 118, 217–226.

Irwin, M. T., Samonds, K. E., Raharison, J.-L., and Wright, P. C. (2004). Lemur latrines: observations of latrine behaviour in wild primates and possible ecological significance. Journal of Mammalogy 85, 420–427.
Lemur latrines: observations of latrine behaviour in wild primates and possible ecological significance.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Kimura, R. (2000). Relationships of the type of social organization to scent-marking and mutual-grooming behaviour in Grevy’s (Equus grevyi) and Grant’s zebras (Equus burchelli bohmi). Journal of Equine Science 11, 91–98.
Relationships of the type of social organization to scent-marking and mutual-grooming behaviour in Grevy’s (Equus grevyi) and Grant’s zebras (Equus burchelli bohmi).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Linklater, W. L., Flamand, J., Rochat, Q., Zekela, N., MacDonald, E., Swaisgood, R., Airton, D. F., Kelly, C. P., Bond, K., Schmidt, I., and Morgan, S. (2006). Preliminary analysis of the free-release and scent-broadcasting strategies for black rhinoceros reintroduction. Ecological Journal 7, 26–34.

López, P., and Martin, J. (2002). Chemical rival recognition decreases aggression levels in male Iberian wall lizards, Podarcis hispanica. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 51, 461–465.
Chemical rival recognition decreases aggression levels in male Iberian wall lizards, Podarcis hispanica.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

López, P., and Martin, J. (2005a). Intersexual differences in chemical composition of precloacal gland secretions in the amphisbaenian Blanus cinereus. Journal of Chemical Ecology 31, 2913–2921.
Intersexual differences in chemical composition of precloacal gland secretions in the amphisbaenian Blanus cinereus.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16365713PubMed |

López, P., and Martin, J. (2005b). Chemical compounds from femoral gland secretions of male Iberian rock lizards, Lacerta monticola cyreni. Zeitschrift für Naturforschung C 60, 632–636.

López, P., Aragón, P., and Martin, J. (1998). Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola cyreni) assess conspecific information using composite signals from faecal pellets. Ethology 104, 809–820.
Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola cyreni) assess conspecific information using composite signals from faecal pellets.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

López, P., Amo, L., and Martin, J. (2006). Reliable signalling by chemical cues of mate traits and health state in male lizards, Lacerta monticola. Journal of Chemical Ecology 32, 473–488.
Reliable signalling by chemical cues of mate traits and health state in male lizards, Lacerta monticola.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 16555130PubMed |

Lund, U., and Agostinelli, C. (2009). ‘CircStats: Circular Statistics, from ‘Topics in Circular Statistics’ (2001).’ R package version 0.2-4. Available at http://CRAN.Rproject.org/package=CircStats [accessed 13 July 2010]

Martins, E. P., Ord, T. J., Slaven, J., Wright, J. L., and Housworth, E. A. (2006). Individual, sexual, seasonal, and temporal variation in the amount of sagebrush lizard scent marks. Journal of Chemical Ecology 32, 881–893.
Individual, sexual, seasonal, and temporal variation in the amount of sagebrush lizard scent marks.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 1:CAS:528:DC%2BD28XkvVOhsbw%3D&md5=f925f9aa8e1f8ccc62c5128a6d3b7910CAS | 16718575PubMed |

Milne, T. (1999). Conservation and the ecology of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard Tiliqua adelaidensis. Ph.D. Thesis, The Flinders University, Adelaide.

Milne, T., and Bull, C. M. (2000). Burrow choice by individuals of different sizes in the endangered pygmy blue tongue lizard Tiliqua adelaidensis. Biological Conservation 95, 295–301.
Burrow choice by individuals of different sizes in the endangered pygmy blue tongue lizard Tiliqua adelaidensis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Milne, T., Bull, C. M., and Hutchinson, M. N. (2003). Use of burrows by the endangered pygmy blue tongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis. Wildlife Research 30, 523–528.
Use of burrows by the endangered pygmy blue tongue lizard, Tiliqua adelaidensis.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

O’Connor, D. E., and Shine, R. (2006). Kin discrimination in the social lizard Egernia saxatilis (Scincidae). Behavioral Ecology 17, 206–211.
Kin discrimination in the social lizard Egernia saxatilis (Scincidae).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Palphramand, K. L., and White, P. C. L. (2007). Badgers, Meles meles, discriminate between neighbour, alien and self scent. Animal Behaviour 74, 429–436.
Badgers, Meles meles, discriminate between neighbour, alien and self scent.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Roberts, S. C., and Gosling, L. M. (2004). Manipulation of olfactory signaling and mate choice for conservation breeding: a case study of harvest mice. Conservation Biology 18, 548–556.
Manipulation of olfactory signaling and mate choice for conservation breeding: a case study of harvest mice.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Roper, T. J., Conradt, L., Butler, J., Christian, S. E., Ostler, J., and Schmid, T. K. (1993). Territorial marking with faeces in badgers (Meles meles): a comparison of boundary and hinterland latrine use. Behaviour 127, 289–307.
Territorial marking with faeces in badgers (Meles meles): a comparison of boundary and hinterland latrine use.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Rostain, R. R., Ben-David, M., Groves, P., and Randall, J. A. (2004). Why do river otters scent-mark? An experimental test of several hypotheses. Animal Behaviour 68, 703–711.
Why do river otters scent-mark? An experimental test of several hypotheses.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Shah, B., Hudson, S., and Shine, R. (2006). Social aggregation by thick tailed geckos (Nephrurus milii, Gekkonodae): does scat piling play a role? Australian Journal of Zoology 54, 271–275.
Social aggregation by thick tailed geckos (Nephrurus milii, Gekkonodae): does scat piling play a role?Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Sneddon, I. A. (1991). Latrine use by the European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Mammalogy 72, 769–775.
Latrine use by the European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Souter, N. J. (2003). Habitat requirements and conservation of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard Tiliqua adelaidensis. Ph.D. Thesis, The Flinders University, Adelaide.

Stewart, P. D., MacDonald, D. W., Newman, C., and Tattersall, F. H. (2002). Behavioural mechanisms of information transmission and reception by badgers, Meles meles, at latrines. Animal Behaviour 63, 999–1007.
Behavioural mechanisms of information transmission and reception by badgers, Meles meles, at latrines.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Swaisgood, R. R. (2007). Current status and future directions of applied behavioral research for animal welfare and conservation. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102, 139–162.
Current status and future directions of applied behavioral research for animal welfare and conservation.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Swaisgood, R. R., Lindberg, D. G., Zhou, X., and Owen, M. A. (2000). The effects of sex, reproductive condition and context on discrimination of conspecific odours by giant pandas. Animal Behaviour 60, 227–237.
The effects of sex, reproductive condition and context on discrimination of conspecific odours by giant pandas.Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar | 10973725PubMed |

Taylor, R. J. (1993). Observations on the behaviour and ecology of the common wombat Vombatus ursinus in northeast Tasmania. Australian Mammalogy 16, 1–7.

Vestal, B. M., Macdonald, D. W., and Da Silva, J. (1978). Comparison of neighbour recognition in two species of deer mice (Peromyscus). Journal of Mammalogy 59, 339–346.
Comparison of neighbour recognition in two species of deer mice (Peromyscus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |

Vilà, C., Urios, V., and Castroviejo, J. (1994). Use of faeces for scent marking in Iberian wolves (Canis lupus). Canadian Journal of Zoology 72, 374–377.
Use of faeces for scent marking in Iberian wolves (Canis lupus).Crossref | GoogleScholarGoogle Scholar |